357 lines
18 KiB
Plaintext
357 lines
18 KiB
Plaintext
March 1991
|
||
|
||
|
||
COGNITIVE INTERVIEWING
|
||
|
||
By
|
||
|
||
Margo Bennett, M.Ed.
|
||
and
|
||
John E. Hess, M.Ed.
|
||
Special Agents and Instructors
|
||
FBI Academy
|
||
Quantico, Virginia
|
||
|
||
|
||
When interviewing crime victims, few investigators begin
|
||
with questions such as: How tall was the subject? What color
|
||
was his hair? Did he have any scars? Common sense, experience,
|
||
and fundamental training lead investigators to the conclusion
|
||
that such specific questions give witnesses little opportunity
|
||
to tell what they know. Instead, open-ended questions tend to
|
||
produce the best results. A question like, "What did he look
|
||
like?" eliminates the need for investigators to anticipate every
|
||
detail of description victims may have noted. Investigators can
|
||
always follow up the witness' statements with specific, direct
|
||
questions to fill in gaps. At least, that is what many
|
||
interview textbooks suggest. But what happens when even these
|
||
direct questions fail to produce the details needed from
|
||
witnesses? The cognitive interview method is a proven
|
||
technique, effective because it provides interviewers with a
|
||
structured approach to help retrieve such details from the
|
||
memories of witnesses.
|
||
|
||
Consider the following scenario: At a robbery scene, a
|
||
uniformed officer briefs the investigating detective. Hoping to
|
||
obtain additional information, the detective approaches the
|
||
clerk, introduces himself, and sensing her anxiety, takes some
|
||
time to assure her that she has nothing to worry about. He
|
||
tells her he understands the trauma she has just undergone, gets
|
||
her a cup of coffee, and delays asking any questions until she
|
||
has regained her composure. He then tells her that he needs her
|
||
help and asks that she start at the beginning and tell him
|
||
exactly what happened. She replies:
|
||
|
||
"I was behind the counter when all of a sudden, I heard a
|
||
voice telling me to give him all the money, and I would not
|
||
get hurt. I looked up and saw a man wearing a ski mask
|
||
pointing a gun right at me. I just froze and stared at the
|
||
gun. He told me to get a move on or there would be trouble.
|
||
I opened the cash register and handed him all of the bills.
|
||
There was just under a hundred dollars in the register. He
|
||
then told me to lie on the floor and not move. I did as he
|
||
told me and waited until I was sure he was gone. I yelled to
|
||
Joe, the manager, who was in the office, who asked me if I
|
||
was okay. He then ran to the phone and called the police.
|
||
The next thing I knew, the police officer arrived, and I told
|
||
him the same thing I just told you. I don't know what the
|
||
guy looked like, where he came from, or how he got away. I'm
|
||
sorry I can't be more help."
|
||
|
||
The detective tells her that she has been very helpful and
|
||
that now he would like to go over the story again, and this
|
||
time, if she doesn't mind, he will interrupt her with questions
|
||
as she goes along. As she retells her story, he constantly
|
||
probes for additional details, such as the possibility of
|
||
additional witnesses, more descriptive data regarding the
|
||
subject and his weapon, words he may have used, noticeable
|
||
accent, and the means of his escape. However, except for a bit
|
||
more descriptive data, the victim was correct; she had told the
|
||
responding officer everything she could remember.
|
||
|
||
THE PROBLEM: INABILITY TO REMEMBER
|
||
|
||
The above scenario illustrates a problem encountered by
|
||
many investigators. That problem results not from investigators
|
||
being unable to ask good questions but simply from witnesses who
|
||
are unable to provide the answers. Responses such as, "I don't
|
||
remember," "That's all I saw," or "I can't recall" frustrate
|
||
many interviewers on a regular basis. In the past, this led
|
||
investigators to try hypnosis as a means of enhancing witness
|
||
recall. Improved results verified what many investigators
|
||
suspected--an inability of witnesses to remember, not a lack of
|
||
observations, was the main problem. (1) Although investigators
|
||
achieved some success through hypnosis, those successes did not
|
||
last long. Courts, on a regular basis, began ruling in favor of
|
||
defense attorneys who alleged that hypnotically elicited
|
||
information may contain flaws and that hypnosis as a means of
|
||
refreshing recall lacks scientific acceptance. (2) Therefore,
|
||
investigators now primarily reserve hypnosis for situations
|
||
where the need for lead information supersedes all other
|
||
considerations. They know full well that using hypnosis will
|
||
probably disqualify a witness from testifying.
|
||
|
||
SOLVING THE PROBLEM: THE COGNITIVE INTERVIEW
|
||
|
||
To enhance witness recall without the stigma attached to
|
||
hypnosis, Ronald P. Fisher and Edward Geiselman, professors at
|
||
Florida International University and UCLA respectively, have
|
||
developed a system they call the cognitive interview. Although
|
||
their process contains few, if any, new ideas, they have
|
||
systematized some techniques which have, for the most part, been
|
||
used by investigators only in a sporadic, piecemeal fashion.
|
||
Research indicates that the cognitive approach to interviewing
|
||
witnesses increases the quantity of information obtained (3) and
|
||
does not jeopardize the witness' credibility in court, as
|
||
hypnosis does.
|
||
|
||
This article compares the traditional interview with the
|
||
cognitive interview. Specifically, this article deals with the
|
||
cognitive interview technique as it assists witness memory
|
||
retrieval by: 1) Reinstating the context of the event, 2)
|
||
recalling the event in a different sequence, and 3) looking at
|
||
the event from different perspectives. It also deals with
|
||
specific retrieval techniques and time factors that affect the
|
||
interview.
|
||
|
||
Reinstate the Context
|
||
|
||
Traditional interviews of victims and witnesses, similar to
|
||
the one described above, usually begin with interviewers first
|
||
taking the time to make introductions and putting witnesses at
|
||
ease before asking, "What happened?" or "What can you tell me
|
||
about...?" Then, specific questions follow that are geared to
|
||
fill in the gaps inadvertently left by witnesses. Proponents of
|
||
the cognitive interview suggest this will not usually produce
|
||
optimum results. Asking people to isolate an event in their
|
||
minds and then to verbalize that event requires them to operate
|
||
in a vacuum. Even without the trauma that often results from
|
||
involvement in a crime, common sense says that human memory
|
||
functions better in context. The cognitive interview process
|
||
takes this into account.
|
||
|
||
What is meant by context and how do interviewers establish
|
||
it? Simply put, interviewers make efforts to reestablish the
|
||
environment, mood, setting, and experiences by asking witnesses
|
||
to relive mentally the events prior to, during, and after the
|
||
crime.
|
||
|
||
Let's return to the robbery scene described above with the
|
||
detective who had already introduced himself to the victim and
|
||
asked for her help. Instead of asking her what happened during
|
||
the crime, using the cognitive interview approach, he proceeds
|
||
as follows: "It's only about 10:00, and it's already been a
|
||
pretty full day for you. How about telling me how your day
|
||
started. Tell me what time you got up, the chores you did, the
|
||
errands you ran and anything else that happened before you came
|
||
to work."
|
||
|
||
As she recounts her activities, he joins the conversation,
|
||
discussing events with her, including the problems of a working
|
||
mother, what she fixed for breakfast, and any other details that
|
||
she mentions. Only when they have developed a clear picture of
|
||
those events does the detective next suggest that the victim
|
||
describe her travel to work. He handles this portion of the
|
||
conversation in the same way. He does not ask perfunctory
|
||
questions geared to getting her quickly to the crime scene, but
|
||
rather, he discusses her commute to work in depth. They discuss
|
||
the route she took, weather and traffic conditions she
|
||
encountered, events she may have noticed, and finally, where she
|
||
parked her car and what she noticed at that time. He wants her
|
||
not only to just describe her day in general but also to relive
|
||
it.
|
||
|
||
He uses the same interview technique regarding her arrival
|
||
at work. By the time they finally get to the discussion of the
|
||
robbery, they have put the event into context. In many
|
||
instances, this process enhances measurably a person's retrieval
|
||
of stored information. Thus, witnesses can see details of the
|
||
robbery in their proper sequence and context. Concentration is
|
||
more focused than during any previous interviews, which may have
|
||
only consisted of isolated questions and answers. The response,
|
||
"I can't remember," will occur less frequently.
|
||
|
||
Change Sequence
|
||
|
||
To continue the interview and further develop the witness'
|
||
recall, another phase of the cognitive interview follows next in
|
||
sequence. Initially, retrieving information from witnesses
|
||
occurs in a normal, chronological flow of events. However, when
|
||
recounting from memory, people tend to edit as memory playback
|
||
occurs. This results in a summary based upon what witnesses
|
||
regard as important. Therefore, interviewers should address
|
||
this problem by prompting witnesses not to hold back even the
|
||
most insignificant detail. Even so, most interviewers can cite
|
||
experiences where valuable information went unmentioned because
|
||
witnesses chose to omit it.
|
||
|
||
By changing the sequence of recall, witnesses can look at
|
||
each stage of the event as a separate entity much akin to
|
||
looking at individual frames from a film. Reverse or
|
||
out-of-order recall also encourages an overly zealous witness to
|
||
stick to the facts. Witnesses find it more difficult to
|
||
embellish the event when they separate themselves from the
|
||
natural flow of events and independently deal with each
|
||
activity.
|
||
|
||
Returning to the eye-witness interview in the opening
|
||
scenario, the detective might continue using the cognitive
|
||
interview technique. Accordingly, he would discuss the
|
||
conversation the victim had with the responding officer and ask
|
||
where she was when the officer arrived. He wants to know
|
||
exactly what she was doing at that time. What did she do
|
||
immediately before that? Through this line of questioning, he
|
||
gradually arrives back at the time of the robbery and before
|
||
hand. Thus, he leads her through a second recounting of the
|
||
crime, only in reverse sequence. This time, her information is
|
||
a collection of pieces, each viewed independently. Just as
|
||
looking at a portion of the landscape may reveal details missed
|
||
while taking in the panoramic view, looking at stages of an
|
||
event may enable witnesses to "see" previously unnoticed items.
|
||
|
||
Change Perspective
|
||
|
||
To further stimulate witness memory recovery, Fisher and
|
||
Geiselman also suggest changing the perspective. (4) Witnesses
|
||
experience an event one time; however, they may perceive it from
|
||
various views. During initial recollection, witnesses
|
||
articulate from their personal perspectives and rarely vary from
|
||
their point of view. By prompting witnesses to physically
|
||
change the positioning in their memories, interviewers give them
|
||
the opportunity to recall more of their experiences. (5)
|
||
Interviewers can change perspective by asking witnesses to
|
||
consider the view of another witness, victim, or an invisible
|
||
eye on the wall.
|
||
|
||
Using the technique of changing the perspective of
|
||
witnesses, the detective in the opening scenario might say: "You
|
||
know those surveillance cameras they have in banks and some
|
||
stores? Too bad there wasn't one on the wall over there. I
|
||
wonder just what it would have recorded; it certainly would have
|
||
had a different vantage point than you did." Through this
|
||
opening statement, he can draw the victim into a discussion of
|
||
what might have been recorded on the nonexistent camera. This
|
||
technique not only provides her with an opportunity to "replay"
|
||
the event from a different perspective but it also serves to
|
||
further detraumatize the situation. Reviewing a film is much
|
||
less traumatic than reliving an armed robbery.
|
||
|
||
SPECIFIC RETRIEVAL
|
||
|
||
Interviewers can use additional techniques to promote
|
||
memory retrieval, depending on the facts of the crime and
|
||
witness information. After witnesses have recounted an event in
|
||
its natural sequence, reverse sequence, and from different
|
||
perspectives, the interviewer can induce specific retrieval by
|
||
asking direct questions. One technique of specific retrieval
|
||
includes associating witness recollection of physical
|
||
appearance, clothing, and sound with something or someone
|
||
familiar to them. Other areas of recall, such as remembering
|
||
names and numbers, may be enhanced by dealing with individual
|
||
components of the item, such as the first letter or number.
|
||
Once established, interviewers direct concentration to the next
|
||
letter or number and build the response.
|
||
|
||
Using this technique, the detective in the robbery scenario
|
||
might have first reviewed the details obtained thus far. At
|
||
certain points, he might have stopped to ask questions such as:
|
||
"You say he had a scary voice. How so? Does it remind you of
|
||
anybody you know, or perhaps somebody you've seen in a movie?"
|
||
"The coveralls he was wearing--ever seen that type before?
|
||
Where? Were they like a pilot's suit, or more like a
|
||
carpenter's?"
|
||
|
||
This context-enhancing technique stems from realizing that
|
||
the victim did not experience this event as a clean slate. She
|
||
had a lifetime of experiences that preceded this activity.
|
||
Therefore, when getting a description of the subject, a
|
||
detective's questions, "Does this person remind you of anyone
|
||
you know? In what way?" likewise provide a context from which
|
||
the victim can make comparisons. This removes her need to
|
||
create, thus enabling her to draw on information with which she
|
||
is comfortable.
|
||
|
||
TIME FACTORS
|
||
|
||
The cognitive interview encourages a witness' in-depth
|
||
retrieval of memory. Success with this technique, although a
|
||
time-consuming process, forces interviewers to avoid some traps
|
||
normally associated with police interviews, specifically,
|
||
rushing the recall of witnesses and interrupting their
|
||
narratives.
|
||
|
||
Witnesses must feel confident that they have time to think,
|
||
speak, reflect, and speak again as often as they need.
|
||
Interviewers can instill this confidence by allowing sufficient
|
||
time for the interview and by refraining from interrupting
|
||
witnesses. (6) All too often, interviewers say, "Tell me what
|
||
happened," but before witnesses speak for 30 seconds,
|
||
interviewers begin interrupting with specific questions. Those
|
||
specific questions should be asked after witnesses have had the
|
||
opportunity to recount the event fully. Allowing time to
|
||
respond also applies when witnesses answer specific retrieval
|
||
questions. Rushing witnesses sends a message to them that their
|
||
information is trivial. This results in witness retrieval
|
||
shutdown. If interviewers don't give them the time, witnesses
|
||
cannot concentrate or remember.
|
||
|
||
The cognitive interview technique not only enhances witness
|
||
recall but also addresses another common problem among
|
||
interviewers--their inability to sustain the interview.
|
||
Interviewers, particularly inexperienced ones, are often reduced
|
||
to saying, "I can't think of anything else to ask. Is there
|
||
anything you're leaving out?" If a witness responds in the
|
||
negative, the interview is over. Using the cognitive technique
|
||
can help interviewers avoid prematurely reaching this point.
|
||
Experience demonstrates that the cognitive interview technique
|
||
allows interviewers to continue discussing events without
|
||
sounding redundant. Indeed, continued conversation in a
|
||
constructive, helpful direction often prompts additional
|
||
information.
|
||
|
||
CONCLUSION
|
||
|
||
Despite significant advances in various forensic fields,
|
||
most crimes are solved by information furnished by people. The
|
||
interview remains the foremost investigative tool for gaining
|
||
information.
|
||
|
||
Although most victims and witnesses try to cooperate, their
|
||
inability to recall vital details can be discouraging, and they
|
||
need help in remembering. This help must come from
|
||
investigators. Merely asking the right questions does not
|
||
suffice; enhancing someone's memory requires active involvement.
|
||
The cognitive approach to interviewing has proven more effective
|
||
than the traditional one by increasing the quality and quantity
|
||
of information obtained from witnesses and victims.
|
||
|
||
|
||
Footnotes
|
||
|
||
(1) John C. Yuille and N. Hope McEwan, "Use of Hypnosis as
|
||
an Aid to Eyewitness Memory," Journal of Applied Psychology,
|
||
1985, vol. 70, No. 2, p. 389.
|
||
|
||
(2) Martin T. Orne, David F. Dinges, and Emily C. Orne,
|
||
"The Forensic Use of Hypnosis," National Institute of Justice,
|
||
December 1984, p. 1.
|
||
|
||
(3) R. Edward Geiselman, Ronald P. Fisher, David P.
|
||
MacKinnon, and Heidi L. Holland, "Eyewitness Memory Enhancement
|
||
in the Police Interview: Cognitive Retrieval Mnemonics Versus
|
||
Hypnosis," Journal of Applied Psychology, 1985, vol. 70, No. 2,
|
||
p. 403.
|
||
|
||
(4) R. Edward Geiselman, and Michael Nielsen, "Cognitive
|
||
Memory Retrieval Techniques," The Police Chief, March 1986, p.
|
||
70.
|
||
|
||
(5) Ibid.
|
||
|
||
(6) R. Edward Geiselman, Ronald P. Fisher, David S.
|
||
Raymond, Lynn M. Jurkevich, and Monica L. Warhaftig, "Enhancing
|
||
Eyewitness Memory: Refining the Cognitive Interview," Journal
|
||
of Police Science and Administration, December 1987, vol. 15,
|
||
No. 4, p. 292.
|
||
|
||
|