1745 lines
94 KiB
Plaintext
1745 lines
94 KiB
Plaintext
|
From: patth@sci.ccny.cuny.edu (Patt Bromberger)
|
||
|
Newsgroups: misc.handicap
|
||
|
Subject: Paper on Reading Disabilities
|
||
|
Message-ID: <27023@handicap.news>
|
||
|
Date: 8 Jan 93 20:00:12 GMT
|
||
|
Originator: wtm@sheldev.shel.isc-br.com
|
||
|
Lines: 1736
|
||
|
|
||
|
Index Number: 27023
|
||
|
|
||
|
NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL BASES OF EDUCATIONAL DISABILITIES
|
||
|
|
||
|
Implications for Diagnosis and Remediation
|
||
|
|
||
|
Expert Paper Submitted to the United Nations
|
||
|
Disability Unit, Vienna
|
||
|
February, 1990
|
||
|
|
||
|
Robert Zenhausern, Ph.D.
|
||
|
Professor of Psychology
|
||
|
St. Johns University
|
||
|
Jamaica, NY 11439
|
||
|
USA
|
||
|
|
||
|
INTRODUCTION
|
||
|
|
||
|
The paragraphs below are in the form of a satire based on the
|
||
|
essay by Jonathan Swift entitled "A Modest Proposal" in which he
|
||
|
presented a solution to the "Irish Problem". The parallel here is
|
||
|
Learning Disability and the inflexible ways these children are
|
||
|
taught. It is the objective of this paper to show that the problem
|
||
|
of the learning disenfranchised is one that can be solved by
|
||
|
increasing the flexibility with which we teach.
|
||
|
Another Modest Proposal:
|
||
|
A Swift Response to an Old Problem
|
||
|
The purpose of this essay is to examine the possibility that we
|
||
|
are systematically doing a disservice to a large segment of the
|
||
|
school population. Students who have auditory or visual
|
||
|
impairments have been allowed to use artificial means, such as
|
||
|
glasses or hearing aids, to correct their deficits. Indeed, it is
|
||
|
considered praiseworthy to identify such problems early and then
|
||
|
use the services of professionals who prescribe optical or
|
||
|
electronic devices which alleviate the deficiencies.
|
||
|
The consequences of such actions, however, have not been
|
||
|
considered fully. Such children may become lazy and make no
|
||
|
attempt to overcome their problems. What motivation will they have
|
||
|
to strengthen their perceptual weaknesses when such devices make
|
||
|
it unnecessary for them to do so? What will such people do if, for
|
||
|
whatever reason, such devices are not available? It is the
|
||
|
contention of this paper that artificial devices are crutches which
|
||
|
interfere with the complete development of the child. As such,
|
||
|
they should be eliminated.
|
||
|
Some might argue (and not without a modicum of validity) that
|
||
|
by eliminating those "support systems," such children may not
|
||
|
progress beyond the elementary rudiments of learning. That,
|
||
|
however, should be secondary to the point that we are not dealing
|
||
|
directly with a serious problem. The fact that our present state
|
||
|
of knowledge does not allow us to correct such deficiencies should
|
||
|
not dissuade us from this course of action. Eventually specific
|
||
|
techniques will be developed to meet the problems of poor eyesight
|
||
|
and hearing in much the same way that techniques were developed to
|
||
|
alleviate reading and mathematical difficulties -- and probably
|
||
|
with as much success. There is a minor problem in the fact that
|
||
|
many of the authority figures in the child's environment use those
|
||
|
same artificial devices and thus do not serve as good role models.
|
||
|
Aside from the educational wisdom of this proposal, it has the
|
||
|
added advantage of eliminating the possibility of charges of
|
||
|
discrimination. Consider, for example, if someone raised the point
|
||
|
that a deficit in vision or hearing might be compared to a deficit
|
||
|
in arithmetic computation. They might argue that if vision can be
|
||
|
corrected by glasses why can a calculation deficit not be corrected
|
||
|
by the use of a calculator? It is difficult to counter these
|
||
|
arguments since the two deficits have so much in common. Even the
|
||
|
poor role model problem has a parallel since most of the authority
|
||
|
figures whom the children contact would have some difficulty in
|
||
|
taking a square root or doing long division of decimals by hand.
|
||
|
The conclusion is clear: take away glasses and hearing aids and
|
||
|
give the children with sensory defects the same advantages given
|
||
|
to children with calculation defects!
|
||
|
* * *
|
||
|
The essay is clearly satirical, but its point is clear. In
|
||
|
this Decade of the Disabled it is essential to consider the human
|
||
|
rights of the Learning Disabled to an education that more closely
|
||
|
fits their capabilities. The problems of the Learning Disabled are
|
||
|
unique because this is the only disabled group which is held
|
||
|
responsible for its disability. "If he worked harder, he could do
|
||
|
it", says the frustrated teacher. No one expects a blind person
|
||
|
to see, if he or she "worked harder". Furthermore, to call a child
|
||
|
"learning disabled" is to put the burden of responsibility on the
|
||
|
wrong person! It is our responsibility to teach much more than it
|
||
|
is the responsibility of the child to learn. It is we who should
|
||
|
be called teaching disabled. The purpose of this paper is to focus
|
||
|
attention on individual differences among both normal and learning
|
||
|
disabled children and to consider alternative approaches to
|
||
|
education and thus eliminate our teaching disability. The major
|
||
|
emphasis will be on the theory and remediation of reading
|
||
|
disability based on a 10 year program of research within a
|
||
|
neuropsychological framework. The initial Chapter will introduce
|
||
|
the concepts of cerebral asymmetry and hemisphericity and put them
|
||
|
in perspective for education today. The second Chapter will
|
||
|
discuss behavioral and physiological measures of individual
|
||
|
differences in neuropsychological functioning. The third Chapter
|
||
|
will describe a study that underlines the importance of these
|
||
|
individual differences in an educational setting. The fourth and
|
||
|
fifth Chapters will describe a series of studies dealing with the
|
||
|
theory, diagnosis and remediation of reading disability that has
|
||
|
been based on these neuropsychological concepts. The final Chapter
|
||
|
will be a summary that includes the basic information on the Direct
|
||
|
Access approach to reading with specific recommendations. It can
|
||
|
serve as an abstract of the whole paper.
|
||
|
This Introduction ends with a short quote, found hanging on
|
||
|
the walls of an elementary school in Greensboro, North Carolina and
|
||
|
attributed to Ken Dunn.
|
||
|
|
||
|
If children cannot learn the way we are teaching them,
|
||
|
then we must teach them the way they can learn.
|
||
|
|
||
|
CHAPTER 1
|
||
|
THE CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES OF THE BRAIN AND THE NEW PHRENOLOGY
|
||
|
|
||
|
Recent work in the areas of neuropsychology, especially that
|
||
|
of Sperry who won the Nobel Prize, has popularized the notion of
|
||
|
cerebral asymmetry. That is, the two hemispheres of the brain are
|
||
|
different in terms of the cognitive processes in which they excel.
|
||
|
There is clear evidence that the Left Hemisphere has unique control
|
||
|
of expressive speech and operates using a sequentially organized
|
||
|
system. The Right Hemisphere, on the other hand, has systems that
|
||
|
are more capable of spatially and pictorially oriented processing.
|
||
|
|
||
|
While there are clear differences between the hemispheres,
|
||
|
these differences have been overgeneralized into a new phrenology
|
||
|
of brain functions. A typical list of "Left Hemisphere Functions"
|
||
|
reads something like: logical, verbal, analytic, inductive,
|
||
|
controlled; the Right Hemisphere is often called: synthetic,
|
||
|
emotional, deductive, intuitive, and abstract. Some of these
|
||
|
labels are self-contradictory. The Left Hemisphere is called both
|
||
|
analytic and inductive and the Right Hemisphere both synthetic and
|
||
|
deductive. The term "abstract" has two diametrically opposed
|
||
|
meanings: an article abstract versus abstract art. These
|
||
|
inconsistencies aside, this neo-phrenological approach must be
|
||
|
rejected on the grounds that it is atomistic. A hemisphere is
|
||
|
neither verbal, logical, emotional, nor creative; it is a person
|
||
|
who has these characteristics! All behavior flows from the
|
||
|
integrated functioning of the whole brain. This does not mean,
|
||
|
however, that there are no individual differences associated with
|
||
|
the brain. The concept of Hemispheric Related Strategies provides
|
||
|
a framework on which to base an individual difference variable.
|
||
|
For the most part, the two hemispheres do the same things but
|
||
|
do them using different approaches. Cerebral asymmetries reflect
|
||
|
relative efficiency rather than a "can do-can't do" dichotomy.
|
||
|
There seems to be one exception to this relative rather than
|
||
|
absolute difference between the hemispheres: for most people, only
|
||
|
the left hemisphere is capable of speech and of phonetic
|
||
|
representation (Levy, 1974). The isolated Left Hemisphere can tell
|
||
|
whether the two words "though" and "blow" rhyme but the isolated
|
||
|
Right Hemisphere can not, even though it may understand their
|
||
|
meaning. Rhyming demands that the written word be converted to an
|
||
|
auditory form and only the left hemisphere has this capability.
|
||
|
A second factor that differentiates the two hemispheres is their
|
||
|
type of processing systems: sequential for the left hemisphere and
|
||
|
parallel for the right hemisphere (see Bradshaw and Nettleton, 1981
|
||
|
for a review.) Compare these two situations: 1) You are given a
|
||
|
description of someone and must then identify that person; and 2)
|
||
|
You are shown a picture of the person and then must select the
|
||
|
person. The first task demands the sequential system of the left
|
||
|
hemisphere--the words, descriptive of the various facial features,
|
||
|
are read in sequence and must be combined into an overall
|
||
|
perception. The latter situation reflects the parallel system of
|
||
|
the right hemisphere -- the picture is seen as a whole and various
|
||
|
facial features can be extracted. The picture is seen all at once
|
||
|
in parallel while the verbal description must be sequentially
|
||
|
processed. The sequential processing system of the Left
|
||
|
Hemisphere and its ability in speech production are the
|
||
|
characteristics that underlie the notion that the Left Hemisphere
|
||
|
is the verbal hemisphere. Language, by its very nature, is
|
||
|
sequential. Word order and syntax are essential to meaning--
|
||
|
language cannot be easily processed in parallel. Thus the left
|
||
|
hemisphere has been labeled the "verbal" hemisphere because its
|
||
|
sequential processing system is compatible with the sequential
|
||
|
nature of language, and its control of auditory linguistic
|
||
|
processes makes it essential for speech.
|
||
|
For other tasks, even though the processing system of either
|
||
|
hemisphere is compatible with the task, one hemisphere is clearly
|
||
|
superior. The example of face recognition shows intuitively that
|
||
|
the Right Hemisphere pictorial approach is better than a Left
|
||
|
Hemisphere written description. Note, however, that the task can
|
||
|
be done using Left Hemisphere strategies, but it takes a thousand
|
||
|
words to describe one picture. Spatial relations tasks are
|
||
|
also more easily handled using Right Hemisphere processes. Other
|
||
|
tasks can be handled equally well using the strategies of either
|
||
|
hemisphere. For example, a list of words could be learned by
|
||
|
converting and storing them as visual representations or in an
|
||
|
auditory form. The term Hemispheric Related Strategies can be used
|
||
|
to describe this relationship between observable behavior and its
|
||
|
underlying neuropsychological bases.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Hemispheric Cognitive Style
|
||
|
Two individuals, when faced with the same task, do not
|
||
|
necessarily use the same strategies, that is, people do things in
|
||
|
different ways. Sometimes those different approaches can be
|
||
|
associated with processing differences between the two cerebral
|
||
|
hemispheres. These different approaches can be termed Hemispheric
|
||
|
Related Traits. Hemispheric Cognitive Style is the tendency of an
|
||
|
individual to use distinct patterns of Hemispheric Related Traits.
|
||
|
It does not imply that one hemisphere is used exclusively, but that
|
||
|
individuals tend to approach tasks in unique and consistent ways.
|
||
|
For example, if a group of individuals were asked to remember the
|
||
|
words "dog, cat, tree, table, chair", few would have difficulty.
|
||
|
If these same individuals were asked what strategies they used,
|
||
|
there would be wide variation. Some would report they repeated
|
||
|
the words to themselves, others that they "saw" the written form
|
||
|
of the word, and others would create images of the words.
|
||
|
These differences in memory strategies can be related to
|
||
|
Hemispheric Related Traits. A person who would be more likely to
|
||
|
use imaginal strategies could be said to use a right Hemispheric
|
||
|
Related Trait, and a person who used auditory strategies could be
|
||
|
said to use a left Hemispheric Related Trait. Some individuals
|
||
|
tend to use the Hemispheric Related Traits associated with one
|
||
|
hemisphere more than those of the other hemisphere and others show
|
||
|
little or no bias. Those who do favor the Hemispheric Related
|
||
|
Traits of one hemisphere can be said to have a Right or Left
|
||
|
Hemispheric Cognitive Style. It is important not to
|
||
|
overgeneralize the scope of Hemispheric Cognitive Style. The fact
|
||
|
that a person tends to use particular strategies implies neither
|
||
|
a disuse nor deficiency in one hemisphere of the brain. On a very
|
||
|
simple level, a right Hemispheric Cognitive Style individual has
|
||
|
access to the speech centers of the left hemisphere just as a left
|
||
|
Hemispheric Cognitive Style individual has access to the prosody
|
||
|
centers of the right hemisphere. Thus both right and left
|
||
|
Hemispheric Cognitive Style individuals rely on the integrated
|
||
|
functioning of both hemispheres for expressive speech, and, in
|
||
|
fact, all behavior. In an intact individual, no task can be
|
||
|
accomplished without the integrated functioning of both
|
||
|
hemispheres. To call an individual "left or right brained" is to
|
||
|
ignore the fact that all activity depends on the integrated
|
||
|
functioning of the whole brain. These differences, however, can
|
||
|
be related to different strategies with which people approach
|
||
|
specific tasks. One purpose of this paper is to show how these
|
||
|
strategy differences can affect the educational system. The next
|
||
|
chapter will describe how differences in Hemispheric Related Traits
|
||
|
can be measured. The following chapters will focus on the
|
||
|
application of these traits to mainstream and learning disabled
|
||
|
education.
|
||
|
|
||
|
CHAPTER 2
|
||
|
THE MEASUREMENT OF HEMISPHERIC COGNITIVE STYLE
|
||
|
|
||
|
Two distinct tools have been used in the measurement of
|
||
|
Hemispheric Cognitive Style, one behavioral and the other
|
||
|
physiological. The behavioral measure is a self rating
|
||
|
questionnaire and the physiological measure relies on the
|
||
|
predominant direction of Lateral Eye Movement (LEM).
|
||
|
|
||
|
The Hemispheric Preference Questionnaire
|
||
|
There are many questionnaires that have been used to measure
|
||
|
"hemisphericity" including many from popular magazines. Over the
|
||
|
past 12 years I have developed an instrument that has been
|
||
|
successfully used to separate right and left Hemispheric Cognitive
|
||
|
Styles. A copy of the questionnaire and its scoring key has been
|
||
|
included. The following studies used the instrument successfully.
|
||
|
Coleman and Zenhausern (1979) compared those who used right and
|
||
|
left Hemispheric Related Traits on a memory retrieval task. They
|
||
|
found the two groups differed on processing speed and the extent
|
||
|
of a left hemisphere bias induced by a verbal memory load. The
|
||
|
bias was four times stronger for the those who use left Hemispheric
|
||
|
Related Traits than for those who use right Hemispheric Related
|
||
|
Traits. Zenhausern and Nickel (1979) found that Right style
|
||
|
individuals learned a finger maze in fewer trials, in less time and
|
||
|
with fewer errors than Left style individuals. Zenhausern, Notaro,
|
||
|
Grosso, and Schiano (1981) presented right and left style
|
||
|
individuals with auditory messages in which there was a conflict
|
||
|
between verbal content and emotional tone of voice. Overall, those
|
||
|
who used right Hemispheric Related Traits responded significantly
|
||
|
more often to the inflection cues and those who use left
|
||
|
Hemispheric Related Traits significantly more often to the verbal
|
||
|
content. Zenhausern and Dunivin (1981) found that left style
|
||
|
subjects were more obsessive compulsive, while right style subjects
|
||
|
had more hysterical traits. Zenhausern and Parisi (1983) have
|
||
|
found that schizophrenics rate themselves as using left while
|
||
|
depressives rate themselves as using right hemisphere related
|
||
|
strategies. The instrument has been used in the area of
|
||
|
reading disability to distinguish two separate syndromes. Oexle
|
||
|
and Zenhausern (1980), Golden and Zenhausern (1981), Zenhausern
|
||
|
and Sinatra (1983), Maxwell and Zenhausern (1983) have found that
|
||
|
85% of reading disabled children rate themselves as using more
|
||
|
right than left hemisphere strategies. A copy of the test and its
|
||
|
scoring key can be found in Table 2.1.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Lateral Eye Movements
|
||
|
Research into the phenomenon of lateral eye movements (LEM)
|
||
|
as a behavioral measure of neuropsychological activity has been
|
||
|
pursued along two separate dimensions. LEM have been considered
|
||
|
a measure of both individual differences and task demands. From a
|
||
|
neuropsychological perspective, the individual difference aspects
|
||
|
have been associated with the concept of cognitive style and the
|
||
|
effects of task demands with hemispheric asymmetry. There is,
|
||
|
however, considerable controversy as to whether LEM do indeed have
|
||
|
neuropsychological relevance. The importance of LEM has been
|
||
|
overgeneralized to the point of faddism, which has led to a general
|
||
|
reluctance on the part of the scientific community to give them
|
||
|
credence. In addition, researchers in the area have sometimes
|
||
|
failed to distinguish between these two different aspects of LEM.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 2.1
|
||
|
The Preference Test for Hemispheric Related Strategies
|
||
|
|
||
|
Indicate your choice by assigning a number from 1 to 10 (with 1
|
||
|
being the lowest) on each question. To score the test, refer to
|
||
|
the scoring key below. Add the ratings for all the items that are
|
||
|
to be scored right and those that are to be scored left, subtract
|
||
|
the two and divide by 10. The larger number shows the predominant
|
||
|
preference and the greater the difference the larger the HRS
|
||
|
preference. You should then develop your own local norms, but as
|
||
|
a rule of thumb a score of .7 or higher can be considered a clear
|
||
|
indication of a preference for a Hemispheric Related Strategy.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Test Items
|
||
|
|
||
|
1) Do you base your decisions on objective facts rather than
|
||
|
feelings?
|
||
|
2) Are you psychic?
|
||
|
3) Do you like using symbols or images in solving problems?
|
||
|
4) Are you artistically or musically creative?
|
||
|
5) Are you logical?
|
||
|
6) Are you good at solving crossword puzzles?
|
||
|
7) Can you read quickly?
|
||
|
8) Are your daydreams vivid?
|
||
|
9) Can you think of synonyms for words easily?
|
||
|
10) Do you remember dreams?
|
||
|
11) Are your dreams vivid?
|
||
|
12) Are you fluent in using words?
|
||
|
13) Are you good at using images in remembering and thinking?
|
||
|
14) Do you use a playful approach to problem solving?
|
||
|
15) Do you use a serious, all business approach to problem
|
||
|
solving? 16) Do you like to keep experiences planned and
|
||
|
structured?
|
||
|
17) Do you like to read or think while sitting upright?
|
||
|
18) How much does your thinking consist of words?
|
||
|
19) How much does your thinking consist of mental imagery?
|
||
|
20) Do you like to explain something using visual presentation?
|
||
|
|
||
|
SCORING KEY
|
||
|
|
||
|
Item Scoring
|
||
|
1 L
|
||
|
2 R
|
||
|
3 R
|
||
|
4 R
|
||
|
5 L
|
||
|
6 L
|
||
|
7 L
|
||
|
8 R
|
||
|
9 L
|
||
|
10 R
|
||
|
|
||
|
Item Scoring
|
||
|
11 R
|
||
|
12 L
|
||
|
13 R
|
||
|
14 R
|
||
|
15 L
|
||
|
16 L
|
||
|
17 L
|
||
|
18 L
|
||
|
19 R
|
||
|
20 R
|
||
|
Individual Differences and LEM
|
||
|
Research into whether LEM
|
||
|
reflect individual personality differences was initiated by Day
|
||
|
(1964), who reported that the direction of LEM was related to
|
||
|
individual styles of coping with anxiety. Bakan (1971) was the
|
||
|
first to propose that the direction in which a person consistently
|
||
|
shifted gaze was related to which of the cerebral hemispheres an
|
||
|
individual used more often.
|
||
|
The relationship between LEM and various dimensions of
|
||
|
individual differences has been explored. Tucker and Suib (1978)
|
||
|
found that left-movers had higher scores on the Performance tests
|
||
|
of the WAIS and did better with imagery oriented questions while
|
||
|
right-movers had better scores on the Verbal subtests of the WAIS
|
||
|
and with questions that were letter and number oriented (e.g., how
|
||
|
many letters are in the word house). Gur and Gur (1975)
|
||
|
showed a relationship between direction of LEM and defensive style.
|
||
|
Predominantly rightward movers more often reported using projection
|
||
|
and "turning against others" as their main defenses, while
|
||
|
predominantly leftward movers reported using repression and denial
|
||
|
more often. LEM were again shown to be related to defensive styles
|
||
|
in males as measured by the Defense Mechanism Inventory (Krikorian
|
||
|
and Rafales, 1983). This effect was not replicated with females,
|
||
|
however (Thompson, Greenberg, Fisher, 1982). In addition, subjects
|
||
|
who moved their eyes bidirectionally rather than predominantly to
|
||
|
the left or the right were shown to have better adaptive coping
|
||
|
styles ratings on the adjective check list (Parrott, 1984).
|
||
|
Smokler and Shevrin (1979) showed that normal subjects with
|
||
|
hysterical tendencies made more leftward LEM than subjects with
|
||
|
obsessive compulsive tendencies. The latter group was more likely
|
||
|
to show rightward LEM. Gur (1978) and Schweitzer (1979) found that
|
||
|
schizophrenics had predominantly leftward LEM.
|
||
|
The relationship between LEM and cognitive styles has also
|
||
|
been explored. Subjects, who scored as left or right style
|
||
|
oriented on the Laterality Preference Schedule, were shown to have
|
||
|
consistent patterns of LEM, indicating the existence of consistent
|
||
|
patterns of information processing (Breitling and Bonnet, 1985;
|
||
|
Bruce, Herman, and Stern, 1982). When using the Your Style of
|
||
|
Learning and Thinking Test (SOLAT) to measure style of thinking,
|
||
|
however, no relationship was found between LEM and thinking style
|
||
|
preference (Alberts and McCallum, 1982). In addition, Owens and
|
||
|
Limber (1983) found no relationship between cognitive style and
|
||
|
LEM.
|
||
|
One area of interest in the cognitive style research is the
|
||
|
relationship between what are considered right style ways of
|
||
|
thinking (holistic and broad) and left style ways of thinking
|
||
|
(analytic and narrow) based on proposed functions of the individual
|
||
|
hemispheres. In support of the theory that eye movements indicate
|
||
|
hemispheric activation, and that left hemisphere activation is
|
||
|
associated with more analytic and narrow styles, Huang and Byrne
|
||
|
(1978) showed that narrow categorizers based on the Pettigrew's
|
||
|
Category Width Scale made more leftward LEM than broad
|
||
|
categorizers.
|
||
|
Another area of interest has been the relationship between the
|
||
|
ability to recall dreams and LEM. Predominantly leftward LEM have
|
||
|
been associated with the ability to vividly recall dreams in male
|
||
|
subjects (Leboeuf, Mckay, Clark, 1983), but the same has not been
|
||
|
found with females (Van Nuys, 1985). A related issue is that of
|
||
|
creativity and LEM. Leftward eye movement has been associated with
|
||
|
thinking of more uses of objects on the Uses Test, which is often
|
||
|
used as a measure of creativity (Falcone and Loder, 1984).
|
||
|
Zenhausern (1987) has shown that LEM can differentiate between two
|
||
|
different types of reading disabled children. Specifically,
|
||
|
rightward LEM are characteristic of reading disabled children who
|
||
|
are unable to derive meaning from the written word despite being
|
||
|
able to say it. Leftward LEM are characteristic of reading
|
||
|
disabled children who are unable to pronounce the word despite
|
||
|
understanding what it means. This sampling of research indicates
|
||
|
the scope of individual differences that have been associated with
|
||
|
LEM. In their critique of the LEM literature, Ehrlichman and
|
||
|
Weinberger (1978) concluded that LEM are reliable measures of
|
||
|
individual differences. They found that despite differences in
|
||
|
methodology and experimental situations the direction a person
|
||
|
moves his or her eyes is a consistent behavior of that individual.
|
||
|
The individual difference studies above indicate their behavioral
|
||
|
relevance.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Functional Hemispheric Asymmetry and LEM
|
||
|
A second perspective on the nature of LEM was introduced by
|
||
|
Kinsbourne (1972) who proposed that LEM reflect the task demands
|
||
|
on the subject. He suggested that those tasks that require input
|
||
|
predominantly from the left hemisphere resulted in rightward LEM,
|
||
|
the direction away from the left hemisphere; those tasks that
|
||
|
required predominantly right hemisphere functions resulted in
|
||
|
leftward LEM, away from the right hemisphere. There has been
|
||
|
mixed support for this relationship between LEM and question type.
|
||
|
The critical variable has usually been whether rightward LEM are
|
||
|
associated with verbal questions, indicating a relationship between
|
||
|
a verbal task and left hemisphere activation. A wide variety of
|
||
|
what have been called "verbal" questions have been used, however.
|
||
|
Galin and Ornstein (1974) reported a relationship between
|
||
|
rightward LEM and logical problems and leftward LEM and visual
|
||
|
imagery tasks. Proverb interpretation has been shown to be
|
||
|
related to rightward LEM (Kinsbourne, 1972; Gur, 1975). Schwartz,
|
||
|
Davidson and Maer, 1977, found a relationship between LEM and task
|
||
|
demands. There have been recent reports of relationships
|
||
|
between both rightward LEM and verbal questions (Hugdahl and
|
||
|
Carlgren, 1981; Ogorman and Siddle, 1981) and leftward LEM and
|
||
|
visual spatial tasks (Swinnen, 1984). In addition to the
|
||
|
verbal/non-verbal dichotomy, emotionally laden questions were used
|
||
|
to elicit predominantly leftward LEM (Krikorian and Rafales, 1983;
|
||
|
Jamieson and Sellick, 1985).
|
||
|
Not all studies have resulted in significant findings. In
|
||
|
fact, Ehrlichman and Weinberger concluded that the evidence for a
|
||
|
relationship between LEM and question type was not convincing on
|
||
|
both empirical and theoretical grounds. From the empirical point
|
||
|
of view, they noted that of the 21 studies reviewed, 10 resulted
|
||
|
in rightward movement in response to verbal questions, and 11
|
||
|
resulted in no difference in LEM to verbal and non verbal
|
||
|
questions. The authors concluded that the literature thus did not
|
||
|
offer strong support that LEM were related to question type.
|
||
|
A re-evaluation of the empirical studies, however, indicates
|
||
|
that there is a consistent relationship found between the type of
|
||
|
question asked and the direction of LEM. In only one of the 21
|
||
|
studies reported by Erhlichman and Weinberger did verbal questions
|
||
|
lead to leftward LEM. While the conclusion that verbal questions
|
||
|
lead to rightward LEM may be in question, the conclusion that
|
||
|
verbal questions do not lead to leftward LEM is strongly supported.
|
||
|
There may be a relationship between task demands and LEM, but it
|
||
|
is not a simplistic "verbal question leads to rightward LEM
|
||
|
relationship".
|
||
|
There is clear evidence that LEM are related to both the type
|
||
|
of question asked and individual difference factors. Therefore,
|
||
|
in any situation, LEM reflect the type of question asked
|
||
|
interacting with the individual differences and it is essential to
|
||
|
separate the two effects.
|
||
|
Two distinct tasks were used in a study by Zenhausern and
|
||
|
Kraemer (1989) to investigage the dual nature of LEM. One task is
|
||
|
purely informational and cannot be uniquely connected with either
|
||
|
hemisphere of the brain. The second task involves rhyming of non-
|
||
|
words which clearly demands the speech centers of the left
|
||
|
hemisphere. Two experimental questions were addressed. 1) Is the
|
||
|
direction of LEM consistent within an individual both across time
|
||
|
and across tasks? 2) Does the type of question asked have an
|
||
|
effect on the direction of LEM?
|
||
|
A total of 50 adult subjects were tested. There were 16 males
|
||
|
and 34 females, with an average age of 27 and a range of 16 to 50.
|
||
|
The stimuli for the rhyme task were four to five letter nonsense
|
||
|
words printed in black ink on white index cards and a series of
|
||
|
prepared questions.
|
||
|
The subjects were informed that they were participating in a
|
||
|
study involving the different ways in which people think. They
|
||
|
were asked two kinds of questions in a face to face encounter with
|
||
|
the experimenter. The subject responded yes or no with a nod or
|
||
|
shake of the head and the initial direction of eye movement to each
|
||
|
of 40 questions was recorded for each subject. These non-verbal
|
||
|
responses were used to eliminate left hemisphere involvement
|
||
|
through speech which was unrelated to the task. The 20
|
||
|
informational questions did not call on any clearly defined brain
|
||
|
areas (Is Miami the capitol of Florida?). The 20 rhyme questions,
|
||
|
on the other hand, clearly demanded the auditory linguistic
|
||
|
capabilities of the left hemisphere. The subject was shown a
|
||
|
nonsense word on an index card and told to remember it. The card
|
||
|
was removed and another nonsense word was verbally spelled by the
|
||
|
examiner and the subjects were asked whether the two words rhymed.
|
||
|
The subjects were tested in two blocks of 20 trials separated by
|
||
|
30 minutes. Within each block 10 informational and 10 rhyme
|
||
|
questions were asked.
|
||
|
The data were first explored by means of a correlational
|
||
|
analysis. Both test-retest and split half reliabilities for
|
||
|
informational questions and for rhyme questions are shown in Table
|
||
|
2.2. In addition, the correlation between rhyme and informational
|
||
|
questions is also presented. LEM are clearly a consistent measure
|
||
|
of individual differences with reliability coefficients averaging
|
||
|
approximately .80 for the same type of question. When the type of
|
||
|
question was changed, however, there was a dramatic drop in the
|
||
|
consistency of LEM although the correlations were still
|
||
|
significant. The direction of LEM changed as a function of task
|
||
|
demands, but not to the extreme that would result in no
|
||
|
correlation.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 2.2
|
||
|
Intercorrelations Among LEM for Information and Rhyme Questions
|
||
|
|
||
|
Split Half Test Retest
|
||
|
Rhyme .85 .78
|
||
|
Information .79 .83
|
||
|
Rhyme with Information .59
|
||
|
|
||
|
An initial analysis on the two 20 question blocks indicated
|
||
|
no significant differences and the two were collapsed into a single
|
||
|
set of questions. The data were then analyzed by means of a split-
|
||
|
plot analysis of variance with LEM Group (whether a subject had
|
||
|
predominantly rightward or leftward movement) as a between factor
|
||
|
and Task (Rhyme and Informational questions) and Direction of
|
||
|
Movement (Right, Left, and Stares) as between factors. Since the
|
||
|
scores are ipsative and the Grouping factor and dependent variable
|
||
|
are related, only the interactions are of interest in this design.
|
||
|
There was a significant interaction between the Direction of
|
||
|
Movement and Task. The results are shown in Table 2.3 below.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 2.3
|
||
|
Number and Direction of LEM as a Function of Type of Question
|
||
|
|
||
|
Task Direction of LEM
|
||
|
Right Left Stare
|
||
|
|
||
|
Rhyme 6.82 7.64 4.92
|
||
|
Information 5.50 7.50 6.50
|
||
|
|
||
|
Simple effects analysis indicated that the number of leftward
|
||
|
LEM to informational and rhyme questions were not significantly
|
||
|
different, but there were significantly more rightward LEM than
|
||
|
leftward LEM to rhyme questions. There were no interactions
|
||
|
involving the LEM Group factor, indicating that the effects of the
|
||
|
type of question were the same for both groups.
|
||
|
These results lead to two conclusions. The first is a
|
||
|
verification of the Ehrlichman and Weinberger (p 1093) conclusion
|
||
|
"...that LEM patterns are reliable characteristics of persons."
|
||
|
Individuals do move their eyes in a consistent direction and
|
||
|
direction of LEM is a reliable measure of individual differences.
|
||
|
The questions, however, remain as to whether LEM differences can
|
||
|
be related to behavior and whether these behaviors have
|
||
|
neuropsychological implications. Some of the research reported
|
||
|
above has shown the scope of the variables that have been related
|
||
|
to differences in LEM. (A fuller listing can be found in Beaumont,
|
||
|
Young, and McManus, 1984). Not every study has shown that LEM
|
||
|
differences were related to differences in performance, but no one
|
||
|
should expect LEM to be related to all behavior.
|
||
|
The final question remains, however, as to whether LEM have
|
||
|
neuropsychological implications. In a very general sense, all
|
||
|
behavior results from brain activity and thus all behavior has
|
||
|
neuropsychological implications. From a more focused point of
|
||
|
view, LEM have been related to behaviors that neuropsychologists
|
||
|
have associated with the brain. The problem has been, however,
|
||
|
that the association of the behavior with the brain has not always
|
||
|
been clearly specified. Logical problems, proverbs, and spelling
|
||
|
have all been used as verbal tasks. These tasks are extremely
|
||
|
complex and clearly involve both hemispheres of the brain, even
|
||
|
though there may be a portion of the task that is especially
|
||
|
dependent on left hemisphere processing. This was clearly shown
|
||
|
in the present study since the rhyming task demanded the unique
|
||
|
ability of the left hemisphere to create the sound of the printed
|
||
|
word. The information task had no such clear connections to either
|
||
|
hemisphere of the brain. Both the correlations and analysis of
|
||
|
variance results pointed to the fact that LEM were different for
|
||
|
the two tasks, with an increase in the number of rightward
|
||
|
movements for the rhyming task. This change, however, was
|
||
|
relatively subtle. Even though the task demanded specific left
|
||
|
hemisphere input, subjects did not make predominantly rightward
|
||
|
LEM; there was only a shift in that direction. A subject who made
|
||
|
predominantly leftward movement continued to do so, but the number
|
||
|
of rightward movements increased and the number of stares
|
||
|
decreased.
|
||
|
Lateral Eye Movements have been shown to be a reliable measure
|
||
|
of individual differences, as well as a response to the type of
|
||
|
questions asked. The importance of both aspects, however, should
|
||
|
not be overemphasized. The individual difference aspect must be
|
||
|
investigated from a more behavioral point of view. Rather than
|
||
|
comparing a lawyer or a scientist with an artist, determine whether
|
||
|
a courtroom lawyer has more in common with an actor or a corporate
|
||
|
lawyer, or whether a geometrician has more in common with an artist
|
||
|
or an algebraist.
|
||
|
The LEM response to task demands must also be considered in
|
||
|
conjunction with the fact that any task demands the integrated
|
||
|
functioning of the whole brain. The variable extent to which a
|
||
|
task places special emphasis on one hemisphere must be considered
|
||
|
within this integrated functioning. LEM can be a useful tool, but
|
||
|
their value and meaning can be misunderstood all too easily. The
|
||
|
next Chapter provides evidence of the usefulness of LEM within a
|
||
|
normal classroom setting.
|
||
|
This chapter has presented the evidence that Hemispheric
|
||
|
Related Strategies can be measured by means of questionnaires and
|
||
|
LEM. The following chapters will provide evidence that this
|
||
|
individual difference variable has significance for education.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Chapter 3
|
||
|
THE INTERACTION OF HEMISPHERIC RELATED STRATEGIES
|
||
|
AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
|
||
|
|
||
|
The purpose of this Chapter is to provide evidence that the
|
||
|
individual difference aspect of LEM has meaningful behavioral
|
||
|
correlates in an educational setting. No assumptions were made as
|
||
|
to the neuropsychological substrate of LEM. They were used simply
|
||
|
as a means of dividing subjects into two groups: those who move
|
||
|
their eyes predominantly to the right and those who move them
|
||
|
predominantly to the left. Half of the right movers were taught
|
||
|
a learning strategy involving verbal rehearsal and the other half
|
||
|
a learning strategy involving imagery. The same procedure was
|
||
|
followed for the left movers. The question was whether there is
|
||
|
a relationship between the typical direction of LEM and the
|
||
|
effectiveness of the two learning strategies.
|
||
|
The subjects were drawn from a pool of 120 average or above
|
||
|
average fifth and sixth graders in a suburban school system. A
|
||
|
series of verbal, spatial, imaginal, and informational questions
|
||
|
were asked in a face to face situation and the 72 subjects who had
|
||
|
the most extreme number of left movements and the most extreme
|
||
|
number of right movements participated in the study. The subjects
|
||
|
were divided into equal numbers of male and female right and left
|
||
|
movers.
|
||
|
Seventy-two words appropriate to fifth and sixth graders and
|
||
|
matched for abstractness, concreteness, and imagery were chosen
|
||
|
from the list provided by Pavivo, Yuille, and Madigan (1968). Two
|
||
|
equivalent sets consisting of 18 pairs, created from this list,
|
||
|
served as the learning stimuli. Two distractor lists of 12 pairs
|
||
|
each were also created using the same procedure. Each pair was
|
||
|
then photographed and developed as 35mm slides.
|
||
|
Each subject, tested individually, was seated approximately
|
||
|
4 ft. from a screen on which the 18 pairs of words were presented
|
||
|
by means of a slide projector for 4 sec. The child was then asked
|
||
|
to recall as many pairs of words as possible and the number of
|
||
|
correct pairs served as one dependent variable. The subjects were
|
||
|
then shown 24 pairs of words, half of which they had already seen
|
||
|
and half of which were distractors. These slides were then
|
||
|
presented and the subject had to indicate by switch closure whether
|
||
|
they had seen the word pair. Both accuracy and response time (in
|
||
|
milliseconds) were measured. Following this pre-training
|
||
|
procedure, half of the subjects whose LEM were predominantly
|
||
|
leftward and half whose LEM were predominantly rightward, were
|
||
|
taught a verbal rehearsal strategy in order to improve performance.
|
||
|
They were instructed to repeat the pairs of words as many times as
|
||
|
possible during the 4 sec interval between pairs. They were then
|
||
|
given six practice trials. The remaining subjects were instructed
|
||
|
on how to form an integrated image from the word pairs. They were
|
||
|
also given six practice trials. The subjects were then tested on
|
||
|
18 new pairs of words using the same procedures as in the pre-
|
||
|
training condition. This effectively created four groups: right
|
||
|
movers taught to use either imagery or verbal rehearsal strategies
|
||
|
and left movers taught the same strategies.
|
||
|
There were three dependent measures that were analyzed in this
|
||
|
study: the number of pairs recalled, the number of pairs
|
||
|
recognized, and the response time for recognition. The design for
|
||
|
all three variables was a split plot factorial with Direction
|
||
|
(leftward or rightward LEM) and Strategy (verbal rehearsal or
|
||
|
imagery) as the between factors and Time (before or after
|
||
|
training) as the within factor. The mean number of correct pairs
|
||
|
recalled under all conditions is shown in Table 3.1.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 3.1
|
||
|
Mean Number of Word Pairs Recalled for Right and Left Movers Under
|
||
|
Verbal Rehearsal and Imagery Instructions
|
||
|
|
||
|
Set Pre Test Post Test
|
||
|
|
||
|
Verbal
|
||
|
Left Movers 3.39 1.78
|
||
|
Right Movers 1.89 2.61
|
||
|
Imagery
|
||
|
Left Movers 2.61 3.28
|
||
|
Right Movers 2.39 2.56
|
||
|
|
||
|
The analysis of variance indicated no significant differences
|
||
|
involving Direction or Strategy, but there was a significant
|
||
|
interaction of Direction, Strategy. and Time. Simple effects
|
||
|
analysis indicated that children with rightward LEM did not change
|
||
|
as a result of imagery instructions, but made a significant
|
||
|
improvement as a result of verbal rehearsal instructions. Children
|
||
|
with leftward LEM made a significant improvement as a result of
|
||
|
imagery instructions, but showed a significant decrement as a
|
||
|
result of verbal rehearsal instructions.
|
||
|
Mean performance for the recognition scores is presented in
|
||
|
Table 3.2. The analysis of variance again indicated a significant
|
||
|
interaction between Direction, Strategy and Time.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 3.2
|
||
|
|
||
|
Set Pre Test Post Test
|
||
|
|
||
|
Verbal
|
||
|
Left Movers 29.28 27.61
|
||
|
Right Movers 28.11 29.44
|
||
|
Imagery
|
||
|
Left Movers 29.56 30.67
|
||
|
Right Movers 29.89 29.44
|
||
|
|
||
|
The simple effects analysis indicated that children with
|
||
|
rightward LEM showed a significant improvement using a verbal
|
||
|
rehearsal strategy and children with leftward LEM showed a
|
||
|
significant decrement using a verbal rehearsal strategy. The mean
|
||
|
reaction times for the recognition task is shown in Table 3.3. The
|
||
|
analysis of variance indicated that there was an overall decrease
|
||
|
in reaction time from pre to post testing and the same significant
|
||
|
three way interaction of Direction x Strategy x Time.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 3.3
|
||
|
Mean Reaction Times (in milliseconds) for the Recognition Task
|
||
|
|
||
|
Set Pre Test Post Test
|
||
|
|
||
|
Verbal
|
||
|
Left Movers 2059 2118
|
||
|
Right Movers 2447 1894
|
||
|
Imagery
|
||
|
Left Movers 2244 1829
|
||
|
Right Movers 1846 1627
|
||
|
|
||
|
Statistical analysis indicated that children with rightward
|
||
|
LEM were significantly faster using a verbal rehearsal strategy and
|
||
|
children with leftward LEM were significantly slower using a verbal
|
||
|
rehearsal strategy. The analyses of the three variables lead to the
|
||
|
single conclusion that children who have predominantly leftward LEM
|
||
|
should not be taught by a verbal repetition strategy.
|
||
|
From the theoretical perspective, these results support the
|
||
|
findings of Ehrlichman and Weinberger that LEM are a reliable
|
||
|
measure of an individual difference variable. It also provides
|
||
|
evidence of the validity of LEM; individuals who differed on LEM
|
||
|
showed differences on a relevant behavioral measure -- verbal
|
||
|
learning. The most important question, however, is whether LEM
|
||
|
have any relationship to brain organization. Individuals who had
|
||
|
predominantly rightward LEM showed better retention when using the
|
||
|
left hemisphere oriented strategy of verbal rehearsal and
|
||
|
individuals who had predominantly leftward LEM showed decreased
|
||
|
retention when using the left hemisphere oriented strategy. These
|
||
|
are the facts, and while it is premature to draw firm conclusions,
|
||
|
these facts are consistent with the existence of a relationship
|
||
|
between LEM and brain organization. The clearest conclusion
|
||
|
from these findings is that children differ in the extent they can
|
||
|
benefit from a verbal repetition strategy in learning. From the
|
||
|
educational perspective it is clear that there are children in
|
||
|
schools who not only do not benefit from a verbal rehearsal
|
||
|
strategy, but whose performance is actually decreased. Spelling
|
||
|
and arithmetic tables are examples of subjects that usually stress
|
||
|
a rote memorization based on oral repetition. One alternative
|
||
|
would be to have the child repeatedly image the letters of the word
|
||
|
or number facts without verbalizing them. When it came time to use
|
||
|
the word or number fact the child would recall the image.
|
||
|
Educators must become more aware of the individual differences in
|
||
|
the way people learn. This becomes even more evident in the next
|
||
|
chapter where these individual differences can be seen to be at the
|
||
|
heart of what has been called reading disability.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Chapter 4
|
||
|
THE DIAGNOSIS OF READING DISABILITY
|
||
|
|
||
|
Although there is general agreement that reading disability
|
||
|
is not a single entity, there is considerably less than a consensus
|
||
|
as to the number of different syndromes that actually exist.
|
||
|
Neuropsychological assessment, the types of errors made with verbal
|
||
|
material, cognitive tests, and differences in processing strategies
|
||
|
have all been used in the classification of the reading disabled
|
||
|
into symptom-related subtypes. As many as five different subtypes
|
||
|
of reading disability have been found through the use of
|
||
|
neuropsychological tests.
|
||
|
For example, Mattis, French and Rapin (1978) reported three
|
||
|
groups and Doehring, Honshko, and Byans (1979) distinguished four
|
||
|
types. Fisk and Rourke (1979) Petroskas and Rourke (1979) have
|
||
|
identified subgroups which were consistent. These subtypes,
|
||
|
however, can be considered in terms of the presence or absence of
|
||
|
auditory linguistic deficits, a distinction reported throughout the
|
||
|
reading disability literature. Subtypes that include auditory
|
||
|
linguistic deficits comprise 80 to 90 percent of the total
|
||
|
population of reading disabled children. The smaller group has
|
||
|
usually been reported to show deficits in visual spatial
|
||
|
processing.
|
||
|
Boder (1973) examined the nature of the spelling errors made
|
||
|
by reading disabled children. She used the term dysphonetic to
|
||
|
describe the type of reading disability marked by linguistic and
|
||
|
phonetic difficulties; and the term dyseidetic to describe the type
|
||
|
which had difficulties with the overall visual spatial aspects of
|
||
|
the written word. Boder estimated that the dysphonetic group was
|
||
|
four to five times more prevalent than the dyseidetic group.
|
||
|
Pirozzolo (1979) used ratings, writing samples, and psychological
|
||
|
and neuropsychological tests to separate two reading disability
|
||
|
groups that were similar to those suggested by Boder. Bakker
|
||
|
(1982) proposed a similar distinction that he related to
|
||
|
hemispheric functioning. The auditory linguistic disabled reader
|
||
|
was not effective in the use of left hemisphere related tasks.
|
||
|
There was also an association of the visual spatial disabled reader
|
||
|
and the effective use of right hemisphere related strategies.
|
||
|
Zenhausern (1987) distinguished these two types on the basis
|
||
|
of both reading related tasks and the predominant direction of
|
||
|
their lateral eye movements. He found that the majority of
|
||
|
children with leftward lateral eye movements had difficulty
|
||
|
determining whether words in their sight vocabulary did or did not
|
||
|
rhyme. He also found that a group of children with predominantly
|
||
|
rightward lateral eye movements had no difficulty in determining
|
||
|
whether two words rhymed, but were deficient in determining whether
|
||
|
words and pictures represented the same concept. The auditory
|
||
|
linguistic group had difficulty converting a word to its sound and
|
||
|
the smaller group showed deficits in converting a word to its
|
||
|
meaning. He used the terms Phonetic and Semantic to describe this
|
||
|
distinction. From a behavioral perspective, phonetic disabled
|
||
|
readers are the children who struggle with every word when they
|
||
|
read aloud and thus lose continuity in the text. They are
|
||
|
frequently anomic and have a general difficulty with the auditory
|
||
|
linguistic aspects of reading, especially the grapheme to phoneme
|
||
|
conversion. The Semantic disabled reader, on the other hand, is the
|
||
|
child who will give a perfect word for word rendition of text, but
|
||
|
has no comprehension of the meaning of that text. The Semantic
|
||
|
disabled readers can convert words into their phonetic
|
||
|
representation, but this representation is not converted into its
|
||
|
meaning. They have no problems with the sound of a word but are
|
||
|
at deficit for tasks involving the meaning of words.
|
||
|
The original study was based on 13 Phonetic and 13 Semantic
|
||
|
readers from the second to fourth grades. One purpose of this
|
||
|
research was to determine whether these Phonetic and Semantic
|
||
|
subtypes would replicate across the entire elementary school
|
||
|
population. A second goal of this study was to replicate the
|
||
|
second finding of the original study. The Phonetic disabled readers
|
||
|
moved their eyes predominantly to the left and Semantic disabled
|
||
|
readers predominantly to the right. This second purpose was an
|
||
|
attempt to determine whether lateral eye movements can be used as
|
||
|
a marker variable for the two types of reading disability.
|
||
|
The subjects in this study were 160 children from the second
|
||
|
to the eighth grades. All were of at least average intelligence.
|
||
|
Forty children were selected at grades 2 or 3, grades 4 or 5,
|
||
|
grades 6 or 7, and grade 8. Of the 40 children at each age level,
|
||
|
20 were at or above grade level and 20 children were at least one
|
||
|
year below grade level in reading. Half of each group were chosen
|
||
|
on the basis of showing rightward LEM and half leftward LEM. The
|
||
|
predominant direction of LEM was determined individually for each
|
||
|
child. A series of 20 informational questions were asked in a face
|
||
|
to face situation and the predominant direction of LEM was noted.
|
||
|
Normal readers split evenly between right and left movers, but 84%
|
||
|
of the disabled readers were left movers.
|
||
|
There were four kinds of reading related tasks that used words
|
||
|
selected from the individual sight vocabulary of each child. A
|
||
|
rhyme task stressed the auditory linguistic aspects of the written
|
||
|
word and the remaining three tasks placed more emphasis on the
|
||
|
meaning of the words.
|
||
|
1) The rhyme stimuli consisted of 10 each of four types of
|
||
|
word pairs: a) words which neither rhymed nor had similar
|
||
|
orthography (tree/eats); b) words which were both phonetically
|
||
|
and orthographically similar (pool/cool);
|
||
|
c) words which were orthographically similar, but did not
|
||
|
rhyme (bone/gone); and d) words which were orthographically
|
||
|
dissimilar, but rhymed (by/tie). 2) The word match stimuli
|
||
|
consisted of 20 word pairs, one in upper case the other in lower
|
||
|
case which did or did not represent the same word (TREE/tree,
|
||
|
TREE/eats).3) The word/picture stimuli consisted of 20 word and
|
||
|
picture pairs, in which the word and picture did or did not
|
||
|
represent the same concept.
|
||
|
4) The synonym/antonym pairs consisted of words which meant
|
||
|
either the same or the opposite.
|
||
|
Each stimulus was presented on 35 mm slides and projected for
|
||
|
130 ms. The subjects were tested individually and responded
|
||
|
verbally as to the whether the words rhymed in the rhyme condition
|
||
|
and whether they matched or meant the same in each of the three
|
||
|
other conditions. All words used in the study were determined to
|
||
|
be in the sight vocabulary of all subjects on the basis of prior
|
||
|
testing.
|
||
|
The number of correct responses for all children on the four
|
||
|
grade levels was subjected to an analysis of variance for each of
|
||
|
the four tasks. The grouping factors included Grade Level, Reading
|
||
|
Ability, and Eye Movement Direction. The normal readers achieved
|
||
|
virtually perfect performance on all tasks and their results were
|
||
|
not included in the tables. The interaction of eye movement group
|
||
|
and the rhyme task was significant. The mean number of correct
|
||
|
responses for the interaction are presented in Table 4.1.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 4.1
|
||
|
Mean Number Correct on the Rhyme Task for Disabled Readers with
|
||
|
predominately Right and Left LEM at Four Grade Levels
|
||
|
|
||
|
LEM Grade Similar Dissimilar
|
||
|
Rhyme Non-Rhyme RhymeNon-Rhyme
|
||
|
|
||
|
Right 2-3 17.80 15.20 14.80 14.00
|
||
|
4-5 17.80 17.80 16.40 17.89
|
||
|
6-7 17.80 18.60 18.60 19.30
|
||
|
8 19.90 19.80 19.10 19.70
|
||
|
Left 2-3 16.10 5.90 7.40 14.40
|
||
|
4-5 16.10 10.30 10.20 17.30
|
||
|
6-7 16.80 13.60 12.50 18.50
|
||
|
8 9.60 12.00 10.40 17.60
|
||
|
|
||
|
On the basis of the simple effects analysis, those children who
|
||
|
had predominantly leftward eye movements were significantly more
|
||
|
impaired than those who moved predominately to the right. This was
|
||
|
particularly true on those conditions for which the orthography and
|
||
|
phonology of the words were inconsistent (bye/tie or bone/gone).
|
||
|
Those children with predominantly leftward lateral eye movements
|
||
|
are the Phonetic disabled readers who have difficulty with the
|
||
|
auditory linguistic aspects of reading. The analyses of
|
||
|
variance for the semantic tasks indicated a significant difference
|
||
|
between disability groups. The results from the three tasks are
|
||
|
presented in Table 4.2. Table 4.2
|
||
|
Mean Number Correct on the Uppercase/lowercase, Word/picture and
|
||
|
Synonym/antonym tasks for Disabled Readers with Right and Left LEM
|
||
|
at Four Grade Levels
|
||
|
|
||
|
LEM Grade Case Word Synonym
|
||
|
|
||
|
Right 2-3 15.70 7.90 8.65
|
||
|
4-5 17.50 7.80 6.55
|
||
|
6-7 18.80 13.40 13.05
|
||
|
8 15.90 15.90 15.00
|
||
|
Left 2-3 19.00 17.50 17.15
|
||
|
4-5 19.30 19.30 17.45
|
||
|
6-7 18.10 19.10 18.75
|
||
|
8 17.40 19.70 17.40
|
||
|
|
||
|
Again the normal readers performed almost flawlessly and the
|
||
|
disabled readers were inferior at every grade level. It was the
|
||
|
reading disabled readers with predominantly rightward LEM who were
|
||
|
the significantly more disabled group for these tasks. They were
|
||
|
significantly inferior on the word matching task, the word picture
|
||
|
task and the synonym antonym task. These children could create the
|
||
|
sound of a word from its orthography, but did not understand the
|
||
|
meaning of that word. This is a replication of a second type of
|
||
|
disabled reader, a Semantic subtype whose deficit involves the
|
||
|
meaning of words rather than their phonology. Rightward lateral
|
||
|
eye movements are a marker for this subtype. In the past, this
|
||
|
subtype has often been identified with visual spatial and
|
||
|
perceptual problems. This may be true but it is incidental to
|
||
|
their reading disability since they had no difficulty in perceiving
|
||
|
the words in the rhyme task. These results support the existence
|
||
|
of two subtypes of reading disability. The Phonetic disabled
|
||
|
reader has difficulty converting the written form of a word to its
|
||
|
phonetic counterpart. The Semantic disabled reader can convert a
|
||
|
word to its sound, but not its meaning.
|
||
|
There were two distinct patterns of errors made by the Phonetic
|
||
|
and Semantic disabled readers, but what is the relationship between
|
||
|
these patterns and reading disability? The answer to this question
|
||
|
lies in the way we teach reading. The next chapter is a discussion
|
||
|
of how these two deficits interact with current reading methods and
|
||
|
the effectiveness of a different approach to the reading process.
|
||
|
|
||
|
CHAPTER 5
|
||
|
THE THEORY AND REMEDIATION OF READING DISABILITY
|
||
|
|
||
|
Current teaching methods almost invariably use an indirect
|
||
|
phonological route to meaning in which the written word is
|
||
|
converted to its phonological counterpart so that meaning derives
|
||
|
from auditory comprehension. In practice, a child comes to school
|
||
|
with auditory comprehension, that is, hearing the word "ball" leads
|
||
|
to the concept of "a round, bouncy thing". In reading, the letters
|
||
|
b-a-l-l must lead to the concept of "a round, bouncy thing". In
|
||
|
virtually every case, the child is taught to see the word, say it,
|
||
|
and understand it from its sound. This is an effective technique
|
||
|
for two reasons. First, it takes advantage of the existing auditory
|
||
|
comprehension of children; second, it provides the background for
|
||
|
the future decoding of new words. As effective as this procedure
|
||
|
is for most children, a significant number of individuals are not
|
||
|
able to learn under this protocol and they comprise the majority
|
||
|
of the children we term "reading disabled."
|
||
|
The Phonetic disabled reader has difficulty with the first step
|
||
|
of this indirect phonological route to reading, converting the
|
||
|
graphemic form of the word into its phonological counterpart. The
|
||
|
Semantic reading disabled readers have no difficulty with this
|
||
|
first step; they can make the grapheme to phoneme conversion. For
|
||
|
whatever reason, however, the sound of the word does not lead to
|
||
|
its comprehension.
|
||
|
The standard methods of teaching reading are well-entrenched and
|
||
|
educators sincerely believe that this indirect phonological route
|
||
|
to meaning is the best. Therefore, remediation for these "disabled
|
||
|
readers" means an intensification of what was not successful in the
|
||
|
past. Extensive drilling in phonetic skills has led to an emphasis
|
||
|
on teaching to weakness, rather than strength. The imbalance is
|
||
|
reflected in reading curricula and standardized tests that stress
|
||
|
phonetic decoding at the expense of comprehension. Phonetic
|
||
|
decoding is a means to the end of comprehension; it has become an
|
||
|
end in itself.
|
||
|
A new approach, Direct Access, has been developed which achieves
|
||
|
comprehension without the necessity of decoding. The Direct Access
|
||
|
method has one basic principle: the meaning of the printed word
|
||
|
should not be derived from the sound of that word. Any procedure
|
||
|
that avoids the grapheme to phoneme conversion is consistent with
|
||
|
this approach. The child is never required to read aloud, but
|
||
|
asked to explain what a passage meant. Trivial deviations in
|
||
|
verbiage, e.g., "jet" for "plane", are de-emphasized. The stress
|
||
|
is on comprehension rather than a slavish word for word decoding.
|
||
|
One of the simplest procedures used in the method involves
|
||
|
pairing words and pictures until the written word triggers a
|
||
|
concept directly, rather than indirectly through its sound. The
|
||
|
child can then construct sentences composed of pictures in parallel
|
||
|
with sentences composed of words. Children automatically fill in
|
||
|
words like "a", "the", "in", etc. when they comprehend the
|
||
|
sentence. In a very short period, the pictures are no longer
|
||
|
needed and the written word is understood on its own.
|
||
|
Several techniques are available for more abstract concepts.
|
||
|
One possibility involves a class discussion of, for example, our
|
||
|
legal system. The teacher can then show the class the word
|
||
|
"justice" and ask the children to draw a picture of justice. It is
|
||
|
not the picture itself that is important, but it serves as a link
|
||
|
between the written word and its conceptualization by the child.
|
||
|
Another possibility is a homework assignment requiring the child
|
||
|
to bring pictures to school that represent specific concepts.
|
||
|
Direct Access places only one constraint on the creativity of
|
||
|
teachers: Do not teach reading by deriving the meaning of a word
|
||
|
from the way it sounds. Phonic decoding is a skill that should be
|
||
|
developed, but it should not serve as the usual reading strategy
|
||
|
for these two groups of readers.
|
||
|
Maxwell and Zenhausern (1983) applied the method to First Grade
|
||
|
children who were "at risk" during the second semester. After 25
|
||
|
half-hour sessions, the children increased their comprehension
|
||
|
scores from the 26th to the 56th percentile on the Metropolitan
|
||
|
Achievement Test and increased their sight vocabulary by over 100
|
||
|
words. A comparable control group showed no gain in comprehension.
|
||
|
Minardi, Zenhausern, and Maxwell (1984) found similar results with
|
||
|
Junior and Senior High School children. Using the same regimen of
|
||
|
25 half-hour sessions, the Junior High School students gained an
|
||
|
average of 7 months and the Senior High School students an average
|
||
|
of 1.4 years.
|
||
|
The previous research on the Direct Access method of reading has
|
||
|
been limited to a small number of children taught by a single
|
||
|
teacher. The purpose of this study was to apply the method in a
|
||
|
large scale basis, using a broad range of grades and teachers.
|
||
|
A total of 209 children from grades 1 through 10 (with the
|
||
|
exclusion of Grade 9) who were at least one year below grade level
|
||
|
in reading and 240 children who were reading at least on grade
|
||
|
level were the subjects in this study.
|
||
|
A workshop explaining the theory and practice of the Direct
|
||
|
Access reading method was presented to teachers throughout a 32
|
||
|
school District in suburban North Carolina. After the workshop was
|
||
|
completed, those teachers who were interested were given further
|
||
|
experience. There were no absolute procedures specified, but the
|
||
|
teachers were shown various possibilities and were told to use any
|
||
|
techniques that did not depend on the indirect phonological route
|
||
|
to meaning. Following these sessions the children in the classes
|
||
|
taught by the teachers were given the Metropolitan Achievement Test
|
||
|
Form L as a pretest measure of their reading ability. The teachers
|
||
|
then used the Direct Access method exclusively for 10 weeks, after
|
||
|
which time the children were retested on the Metropolitan
|
||
|
Achievement Test Form M.
|
||
|
The results of the reading disabled children and a control group
|
||
|
are presented in Table 3. The average gain across the whole group
|
||
|
was almost 1 year and several grades showed gains of over 2 years.
|
||
|
Individual gains of 4 or more years were not uncommon. Given that
|
||
|
these results were obtained during only a 10-week period, the
|
||
|
Direct Access approach is clearly an effective strategy to use with
|
||
|
Phonetic and Semantic reading disabled children. In addition to
|
||
|
these objective gains, the teachers reported an extremely positive
|
||
|
reaction on the part of the students and have commented on the
|
||
|
effects of the method on both spelling and writing composition.
|
||
|
During the past year (1988-89) Greensboro and High Point, North
|
||
|
Carolina used the Direct Access approach. The average gain on the
|
||
|
State mandated California Achievement Test was over 20 percentile
|
||
|
points for those children. As a result, High Point has mandated
|
||
|
the Direct Access approach as the treatment of choice for children
|
||
|
who are "at risk" for reading. The approach has been used in
|
||
|
Currituck, North Carolina since September, 1989 and at Public
|
||
|
School 102 in New York City since January, 1990. There are plans
|
||
|
for incorporating Direct Access into a psychiatric hospital for
|
||
|
children, a Parochial school in New York City, and a school system
|
||
|
in East Greenwich, Rhode Island by February, 1990.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Table 1
|
||
|
Total Gains in Percentile and Grade Equivalent Scores for Direct
|
||
|
Access and Controls
|
||
|
|
||
|
Pre Post Gain
|
||
|
Per Grade Per Grade Per Grade
|
||
|
|
||
|
Grade 1
|
||
|
DA (38) 7 1.09 32 1.55 25 0.46
|
||
|
Control (27) 46 1.79 56 2.13 10 0.34
|
||
|
Grade 2
|
||
|
DA (21) 17 1.76 32 2.15 15 0.39
|
||
|
Control (36) 54 2.89 58 3.19 40 0.3
|
||
|
|
||
|
Grade 3
|
||
|
DA (28) 16 2.33 31 2.99 15 0.66
|
||
|
Control (28) 56 4.49 61 4.94 5 0.45
|
||
|
|
||
|
Grade 4
|
||
|
DA (52) 15 2.59 31 3.28 16 0.69
|
||
|
Control (53) 60 5.82 61 5.97 1 0.15
|
||
|
Grade 5
|
||
|
DA (20) 21 3.63 38 5.01 17 1.38
|
||
|
Control (38) 56 6.53 60 7.09 4 0.56
|
||
|
|
||
|
Grade 6
|
||
|
DA (13) 3 2.36 15 3.68 12 1.32
|
||
|
Control (17) 71 9.58 74 9.9 3 0.32
|
||
|
Grade 7
|
||
|
DA (21) 8 3.39 26 5.45 18 2.068
|
||
|
Control (18) 35 6.57 36 6.64 1 0.07
|
||
|
Grade 8
|
||
|
DA (9) 5 3.38 21 5.61 16 2.23
|
||
|
Control (10) 34 6.94 35 7.36 1 0.42
|
||
|
Grade 10
|
||
|
DA (7) 17 5.58 26 6.65 9 1.07
|
||
|
Control (13) 15 5.25 18 5.6 3 0.35
|
||
|
|
||
|
Total
|
||
|
Exper.(209) 12 2.27 29 3.22 17 0.95
|
||
|
Control (240) 52 5.35 56 5.71 4 0.36
|
||
|
|
||
|
This chapter has outlined some of the successes that have been
|
||
|
attributed to the Direct Access approach. The next Chapter is a
|
||
|
discussion of some of the methods that are consistent with Direct
|
||
|
Access.
|
||
|
|
||
|
CHAPTER 6
|
||
|
SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, RECOMMENDATIONS
|
||
|
Summary
|
||
|
Current teaching methods almost invariably use an indirect
|
||
|
phonological route to meaning in which the written word is
|
||
|
converted to its phonological counterpart so that meaning derives
|
||
|
from auditory comprehension. A child comes to school knowing that
|
||
|
the sound "ball" means the concept "a round, bouncy thing". In
|
||
|
reading, the letters b-a-l-l must lead to the concept of "a round,
|
||
|
bouncy thing". In virtually every case, the child is taught to see
|
||
|
the word, say it, and understand its meaning from its sound. This
|
||
|
is an effective technique for two reasons. First, it takes
|
||
|
advantage of the existing auditory comprehension of children;
|
||
|
second, it provides the background for the future decoding of new
|
||
|
words. As effective as this procedure is for most children, a
|
||
|
significant number of individuals are not able to learn under this
|
||
|
protocol and they comprise the majority of the children we term
|
||
|
"reading disabled."
|
||
|
Kaliski, Zenhausern, and Andrews have shown that there are two
|
||
|
groups of children who have unique deficits that interact with
|
||
|
these standard strategies used for teaching reading. It is this
|
||
|
interaction that directly leads to reading disability. All the
|
||
|
children in grades 1 to 8 who were reading at least one year below
|
||
|
grade level were screened for inclusion in the study. The majority
|
||
|
of the reading disabled group (85 per cent) fall into the category
|
||
|
of children who have to struggle to pronounce every word and thus
|
||
|
lose all continuity in reading. They were termed Phonetic disabled
|
||
|
readers. The smaller group of reading disabled children can "read"
|
||
|
aloud fluently, but do not comprehend what was "read". These
|
||
|
children were called Semantic disabled readers. The final sample
|
||
|
consisted of 80 children, 40 Phonetic and 40 Semantic disabled
|
||
|
readers, spread evenly across the 8 grades. The authors showed
|
||
|
that the Phonetic children could not determine whether two words
|
||
|
(which were known to be in their sight vocabulary) did or did not
|
||
|
rhyme. The Semantic group had no difficulty with a rhyme task, but
|
||
|
made considerably more errors than the normal readers and Phonetic
|
||
|
disabled readers in determining whether a word and a picture
|
||
|
represented the same concept. These two deficits can be directly
|
||
|
related to the indirect phonological approaches to the teaching of
|
||
|
reading. The Phonetic disabled reader can not convert the word to
|
||
|
its sound and thus can not take the first step required by these
|
||
|
reading methods. The Semantic disabled reader can perceive the
|
||
|
words and translate them to their sound but has difficulty in
|
||
|
comprehending the meaning of the written word from this sound. This
|
||
|
disabled reader can take the first step and convert the written
|
||
|
word to its phonological counterpart, but the sound does not lead
|
||
|
to meaning. The results of this study can be seen in Figures 1 and
|
||
|
2.
|
||
|
|
||
|
The standard methods of teaching reading are well-entrenched
|
||
|
and educators sincerely believe that this indirect phonological
|
||
|
route to meaning is the best. Therefore, remediation for these
|
||
|
"disabled readers" means an intensification of what was not
|
||
|
successful in the past. Extensive drilling in phonetic skills has
|
||
|
led to an emphasis on teaching to weakness, rather than strength.
|
||
|
The imbalance is reflected in reading curricula and standardized
|
||
|
tests that stress phonetic decoding at the expense of
|
||
|
comprehension. Phonetic decoding is a means to the end of
|
||
|
comprehension; it has become an end in itself. An alternative
|
||
|
approach to reading which does not depend on an indirect
|
||
|
phonological approach to comprehension, called Direct Access, has
|
||
|
one basic principle: the meaning of the printed word should not be
|
||
|
derived from the sound of that word. Any procedure that avoids the
|
||
|
grapheme to phoneme conversion is consistent with this approach.
|
||
|
The child is never required to read aloud, but is asked to explain
|
||
|
what a passage meant. Trivial deviations in verbiage, e.g., "jet"
|
||
|
for "plane", are de-emphasized. The stress is on comprehension
|
||
|
rather than a slavish word for word decoding. Specific Direct
|
||
|
Access techniques will be discussed later in this chapter.
|
||
|
The first study involving Direct Access was by Maxwell and
|
||
|
Zenhausern (1982) who applied the approach to First Grade children
|
||
|
"at risk" in reading by the second half of the year. After 25 half-
|
||
|
hour sessions, the children increased their scores from the 26th
|
||
|
to the 56th percentile on the Metropolitan Achievement Test and
|
||
|
increased their sight vocabulary by over 100 words. A comparable
|
||
|
control group showed no gain. Minardi, Zenhausern, and Maxwell
|
||
|
(1983) found similar results with Junior and Senior High School
|
||
|
children. Using the same regimen of 25 half-hour sessions, the
|
||
|
Junior High School students gained an average of 7 months and the
|
||
|
Senior High School students an average of 1.4 years on the Reading
|
||
|
Comprehension scale of the Metropolitan Achievement Test.
|
||
|
During the Spring of 1988, over 200 reading disabled children
|
||
|
in grades 1 through 10 from 8 schools within the Guilford County
|
||
|
School System in Greensboro NC used the Direct Access approach.
|
||
|
The children were tested on the Metropolitan Achievement Test
|
||
|
before and after 10 weeks of using the approach. The average gain
|
||
|
was .95 years and every grade from Grade 6 onward gained at least
|
||
|
1 year, with the 7th and 8th grades showing gains of over 2 years.
|
||
|
In the Fall of 1988, High Point NC used the approach with 73
|
||
|
reading disabled children. After 7 weeks, there was an average
|
||
|
gain on the Woodcock Johnson of 15 months. The Kindergarten group
|
||
|
showed a 12 month gain; the primary, a 7 month gain, and the middle
|
||
|
school a 21 month gain. Both Greensboro and High Point, North
|
||
|
Carolina used Direct Access for the past year. On their State
|
||
|
mandated California Achievement Test, those children on Direct
|
||
|
Access instruction showed an average gain of 17 percentile points
|
||
|
above last year's scores.
|
||
|
|
||
|
SELECTED DIRECT ACCESS TECHNIQUES
|
||
|
The results of the demonstration project and the use of Direct
|
||
|
Access in the past year in North Carolina clearly show that reading
|
||
|
disabled children can benefit from the Direct Access method of
|
||
|
teaching reading. To be able to read means that one can get
|
||
|
meaning from the printed word. The indirect phonological route
|
||
|
attempts to accomplish this by having the reader convert the
|
||
|
written word to its phonological counterpart and from this sound
|
||
|
derive meaning. Converting the word to its sound is a means to the
|
||
|
end of comprehension, not the comprehension itself. Direct Access
|
||
|
attempts to derive the meaning of the printed word not via its
|
||
|
sound, but directly. The stress is on comprehension, not decoding.
|
||
|
One of the first and most basic Direct Access techniques
|
||
|
consists of pairing an index card which contains a word and one
|
||
|
that contains a picture so that the child can consistently make the
|
||
|
pairing. At that point the child can read the word, that is, the
|
||
|
child can comprehend the printed word. Note that this is true
|
||
|
whether or not the child can say the word correctly or not. The
|
||
|
pictures can be provided by the teacher, cut from magazines for
|
||
|
homework by the child, or even drawn by the child. The question
|
||
|
of abstractions and hard to picture words at first seems
|
||
|
insurmountable, but it is surprising how easily a child will
|
||
|
develop and remember appropriate pictures. In connection with this
|
||
|
aspect, games can be developed to strengthen the connection between
|
||
|
the words and the pictures. Variations of Concentration and Old
|
||
|
Maid have been used successfully.
|
||
|
This pairing, however, is only the beginning of the Direct
|
||
|
Access approach. The typical reaction at this point is, "This is
|
||
|
nothing new." or "We tried that 20 years ago and it did not work."
|
||
|
The next step is the most critical. The words and pictures must
|
||
|
be combined into sentences. A word sentence can be covered by the
|
||
|
appropriate picture and vice versa. The child should not be asked
|
||
|
to read the sentence aloud, word for word. Rather, have the child
|
||
|
summarize the sentence, point to a picture from a series that
|
||
|
corresponds to it, or even draw a picture of the sentence. The
|
||
|
stress should always be on whether the child has understood the
|
||
|
basic meaning of the sentence rather than a word for word rendition
|
||
|
of it.
|
||
|
There are two important side benefits of this approach. One
|
||
|
is an increase in spelling skills. The second advantage is the
|
||
|
ease with which children can do creative writing using the cards.
|
||
|
It is a simple way to separate penmanship from writing. The child
|
||
|
could be asked to write the funniest (most exciting, etc.) sentence
|
||
|
they can from their words. The step from sentences to paragraphs
|
||
|
is minimal.
|
||
|
There are two basic approaches to class recitation within a
|
||
|
Direct Access framework. The phonetic disabled reader would be
|
||
|
asked to summarize a passage for the class and the teacher and
|
||
|
classmates could discuss the errors of omission and commission.
|
||
|
It also opens the door for class discussion on any relevant issues.
|
||
|
The semantic disabled reader would read aloud, but be required to
|
||
|
do so with expression to assure that the meaning and not only the
|
||
|
words were being read. Of course, any child could be given the
|
||
|
opportunity to use either approach.
|
||
|
Flash cards are a standard part of the classroom and can be
|
||
|
used within a Direct Access framework. Let the children have their
|
||
|
pictures in front of them when the words are flashed. They will
|
||
|
serve as a cue and minimize the chance of failure. As the children
|
||
|
progress, they can turn the pictures face down on the desk and only
|
||
|
turn them over as needed. This will strengthen skills and continue
|
||
|
to minimize the chance of failure. This technique could easily be
|
||
|
converted into a game where more points accrue if a card is not
|
||
|
turned over, but the child will always get some points.
|
||
|
What happens when children come across a word they have never
|
||
|
seen before? This is the usual response of people who first
|
||
|
encounter Direct Access. There are several approaches to this.
|
||
|
First and foremost, the child should be encouraged to determine the
|
||
|
meaning of the word from the context of the whole passage.
|
||
|
Second, the teacher can tell the child the word and immediately
|
||
|
have the child cut out or draw an appropriate picture for that
|
||
|
word.
|
||
|
Third, the child can be encouraged to look it up in the
|
||
|
dictionary just like anyone else who comes across a word with which
|
||
|
they are unfamiliar. The child may not be able to pronounce the
|
||
|
word, but its meaning will be known.
|
||
|
Fourth, phonetic decoding should be an essential part of any
|
||
|
reading program. It is an important skill and should not be
|
||
|
neglected. On the other hand, it should not be the main approach
|
||
|
to deriving meaning from the printed word for some children.
|
||
|
There is a temporary remediation procedure to use with Semantic
|
||
|
disabled readers. Have the child read aloud into a tape recorder.
|
||
|
Then the child can play back the tape while reading from the text
|
||
|
material. Although this is an awkward procedure it will allow the
|
||
|
child to get meaning from the printed word. Direct Access has a
|
||
|
technique for the teaching of spelling and number facts. It has
|
||
|
been shown that some children not only do not learn by verbally
|
||
|
repeating the word and letters (e.g. "cat, C-A-T, cat", "6 times
|
||
|
3 is 18") but this procedure can actually interfere with learning.
|
||
|
As an alternative have the child look at the word and practice
|
||
|
forming an image of the letters without saying anything (This is
|
||
|
essential!!). When it comes time to spell the word, have the child
|
||
|
bring back the image of the letters and copy them.
|
||
|
The Direct Access approach does not depend on a rigid structure
|
||
|
that the teacher must follow slavishly, but is a general principle
|
||
|
that can lead to unique and creative techniques. Furthermore, the
|
||
|
method is not limited to the disabled population, but may be an
|
||
|
effective strategy for all readers since it has some similarities
|
||
|
to "speed reading" techniques.
|
||
|
|
||
|
SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS
|
||
|
Summary
|
||
|
Reading Disability is as much a physical disability as
|
||
|
blindness, deafness and paralysis but a person with such a
|
||
|
disability has not received the help offered to those suffering
|
||
|
from the latter disorders. The reading disabled child is
|
||
|
considered responsible for the disability. The purpose of this
|
||
|
paper is to point out that these children can learn to read if only
|
||
|
we are willing to change the methods with which we teach reading.
|
||
|
|
||
|
The standard approaches to reading demand that the child
|
||
|
convert the written word to its sound and, from that sound, derive
|
||
|
the meaning. It was shown that 85% of the children we call reading
|
||
|
disabled have difficulty making that first step and the remaining
|
||
|
15% can convert the word to its sound, but this still does not give
|
||
|
them meaning. The Direct Access approach to reading can help both
|
||
|
types of children because meaning is not derived by converting the
|
||
|
printed word to its sound. Rather, the sound of the word is
|
||
|
derived from its meaning. Several specific techniques using this
|
||
|
approach have been discussed earlier in this Paper.
|
||
|
Our brain is capable of many fascinating and wondrous things.
|
||
|
Our conscious awareness of our surroundings is somehow derived from
|
||
|
light of various wavelengths falling on our retina and causing
|
||
|
neurons to fire and not fire. It is this pattern of neuronal
|
||
|
activity that gives us conscious experience. Even more amazing is
|
||
|
the ability to create literary, musical, and visual works of art.
|
||
|
It is the brain that is responsible for all of this. The brain
|
||
|
also has a rather obscure function. It can take an arbitrary
|
||
|
series of symbols (printed words) and convert them into an equally
|
||
|
arbitrary set of sounds (spoken words). This function pales
|
||
|
alongside literary and artistic masterpieces. Why have we made it
|
||
|
the basis of our educational system?
|
||
|
Recommendations
|
||
|
There are two major administrative recommendations that flow
|
||
|
from this paper. The first concerns the establishment of a centre
|
||
|
for learning disability to be associated with the Centre for Social
|
||
|
Development and Humanitarian Affairs. The second concerns the
|
||
|
development of a pilot project to investigate the effectiveness of
|
||
|
the Direct Access approach to reading.
|
||
|
1) A center for the dissemination of information and training in
|
||
|
learning disabilities should be established which would be called
|
||
|
the Learning Enhancement Centre of the Disabled Persons Unit.
|
||
|
The purpose of the Learning Enhancement Centre would be to
|
||
|
research and disseminate information on the theory and remediation
|
||
|
of educational disabilities and to provide training in the
|
||
|
remediation of these disabilities. The scope of this training
|
||
|
would include all educational approaches that stress the individual
|
||
|
styles of learning with a particular emphasis on the Direct Access
|
||
|
approach in reading. There would be two main educational foci:
|
||
|
1) ongoing workshops open to teachers from all member
|
||
|
countries; and 2) visiting teams which would provide on-site
|
||
|
training for those teachers who cannot travel to the Centre. In
|
||
|
addition, new teaching techniques would constantly be developed
|
||
|
and evaluated in real classroom situations. These teaching
|
||
|
techniques would take advantage of the latest technology, but the
|
||
|
effective use of more basic strategies would also be investigated.
|
||
|
The Centre would publish a newsletter to promulgate its activities
|
||
|
and share the latest innovations in teaching techniques.
|
||
|
2) A large scale pilot study should be initiated by the Learning
|
||
|
Enhancement Centre to evaluate the effectiveness of the Direct
|
||
|
Access approach across a wide variety of cultures. The Learning
|
||
|
Enhancement Centre would provide a series of workshops to train
|
||
|
teachers in the theory and practice of Direct Access. These
|
||
|
teachers would consist of individuals from various countries who
|
||
|
would then develop Direct Access projects in their country.
|
||
|
Consultants from the Learning Enhancement Centre would visit the
|
||
|
project sites to provide ongoing feedback to the teachers. These
|
||
|
same teachers can provide on site training for countries who are
|
||
|
unable to send teachers to the Centre. In addition to these
|
||
|
administrative recommendations, there are several educational
|
||
|
recommendations that are aimed at eliminating all forms of learning
|
||
|
disability. The aim of the Learning Enhancement Centre is to
|
||
|
implement these recommendations. 3) Individual preferences in
|
||
|
learning strategies should be taken into consideration in selecting
|
||
|
teaching strategies.
|
||
|
Both lateral eye movements and Hemispheric Preference Test data
|
||
|
should be collected on all children. Strategies of teaching that
|
||
|
are compatible with the learning strategies associated with these
|
||
|
Hemispheric Related Strategies should be incorporated into the
|
||
|
classroom. In addition, other measures of individual differences
|
||
|
in learning styles should be incorporated into the framework of the
|
||
|
Centre.
|
||
|
4) A testing program aimed at identifying individual differences
|
||
|
in learning styles should be developed at the Centre and
|
||
|
disseminated among the various member nations. Children can be
|
||
|
tested for Phonetic and Semantic reading disability by means of a
|
||
|
test derived from the results presented in Chapter 4. The Phonetic
|
||
|
Semantic Reading Scale (PSRS) requires a child to match written and
|
||
|
pictorial material on the basis of their sound or on the basis of
|
||
|
their meaning. The point would be to identify individuals who do
|
||
|
well on one portion of the test and poorly on the other to make a
|
||
|
differential diagnosis between the Phonetic and Semantic disabled.
|
||
|
|
||
|
5) The techniques described in the Selected Direct Access Technique
|
||
|
section above should be applied to these children.
|
||
|
These would include, but not be limited to:
|
||
|
a) Alternative forms of class recitation
|
||
|
b) De-emphasis of phonics
|
||
|
c) Use of word picture vocabulary cards
|
||
|
d) Emphasis on comprehension rather than decoding
|
||
|
6) The criteria for success should reflect the capabilities of the
|
||
|
child in conjunction with the demands of the culture rather than
|
||
|
artificial restrictions.
|
||
|
These would include, but not be limited to:
|
||
|
a) Speed should be de-emphasized
|
||
|
b) Tests should be given on an "open book" basis.
|
||
|
c) Calculators should be allowed on mathematics tests.
|
||
|
Note that these changes actually reflect the way people operate
|
||
|
in the real world. No successful business places unrealistic time
|
||
|
limits on workers or denies them use of tools and references.
|
||
|
7) An essential aspect of the Centre will be to develop new and
|
||
|
creative techniques aimed at the elimination of all forms of
|
||
|
learning disabilities.
|
||
|
While the emphasis at present is on developmental disabilities,
|
||
|
it is expected that this will expand to cover acquired
|
||
|
disabilities, especially those related to head injuries. The same
|
||
|
principles of educational remediation will be applied to cognitive
|
||
|
remediation. The major recommendation of this paper is that
|
||
|
learning disability should be recognized as a true disability and
|
||
|
treated in the same way as the more physical disabilities such as
|
||
|
deafness, blindness and paralysis. The learning disabled child
|
||
|
should not be considered at fault for the disorder, but should be
|
||
|
recognized as a person in need of special help. It is our
|
||
|
responsibility to supply that special help.
|
||
|
The emphasis must be on individualizing instruction rather than
|
||
|
finding the "one true method". The good from all approaches must
|
||
|
be used with those who find it good and avoided for those who find
|
||
|
it not good. Only by tailoring our teaching to the unique style
|
||
|
of each student do we maximize their strengths rather than trying
|
||
|
to strengthen their minima.
|
||
|
|
||
|
BIBLIOGRAPHY
|
||
|
|
||
|
Alberts, F. L., & McCallum, R. S. (1982). The relationship among
|
||
|
three measures of cognitive style. Clinical Neuropsychology,4,
|
||
|
70-71.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Bakker, D. (1982) Cognitive deficits and cerebral asymmetry.
|
||
|
Journal of Research and Development in Education, 150, 49-54.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Beaumont, J., Young, A., & McManus (1984) Hemisphericity: A
|
||
|
critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 191-212.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Boder, E. (1973) Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach
|
||
|
based on three atypical reading-spelling patterns.
|
||
|
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 663- 687.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Bradshaw, J. & Nettleton, N. (1981) The nature of hemispheric
|
||
|
specialization in man. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 4, 51-97
|
||
|
|
||
|
Breitling, D., & Bonnet, K. (1985). Lateralization of GSR, lateral
|
||
|
eye movements and a visual half-fields recognition task.
|
||
|
International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7, 140-143
|
||
|
|
||
|
Bruce, P. R., Herman, J. F., & Stern, J. (1982). Lateral eye
|
||
|
movements and the recall of spatial information in a familiar,
|
||
|
large-scale environment. Neuropsychologica, 20, 505-508.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Coleman, S. & Zenhausern, R. (1979) Processing speed, laterality
|
||
|
patterns, and memory encoding as a function of hemispheric
|
||
|
dominance. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 14, 357-360.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Day, M. E. (1967) An eye-movement indicator of individual
|
||
|
differences in the psychological organization of attentional
|
||
|
process and anxiety. Journal of Psychology, 66, 51-62.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Denckla, M.B. (1979) Childhood learning disabilities. In. K.M.
|
||
|
Heilman and E.M. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical Neuropsychology,
|
||
|
New York: Oxford University Press.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Dunn, R., Price, G., Bacilious, Z., & Zenhausern, R. (1983)
|
||
|
Learning Style -- a predictor of school achievement. Momentum,
|
||
|
13, 47-48.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Dunn, R., Zenhausern, R., Cavanaugh, D., & Eberle, B. (1982)
|
||
|
Hemispheric preference the newest elements of learning style.
|
||
|
Journal of the National Association of Biology Teachers, 44,
|
||
|
291-294.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Dunn, R, White, R. & Zenhausern, R. (1984) Do educators create some
|
||
|
discipline problems? Illinois School Research and Development
|
||
|
Journal, 34, 56-67.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Ehrlichman, H., & Barrett, J. (1984) Right
|
||
|
hemisphere specialization for mental imagery: A review of the
|
||
|
evidence. Brain and Cognition, 2, 55-76.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Ehrlichman, H., & Weinberger, A. (1978) Lateral eye movements and
|
||
|
hemispheric asymmetry: A critical review. Psychological
|
||
|
Bulletin, 86, 1080-1101.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Falcone, D. J., & Loder, K. (1984). A
|
||
|
modified lateral eye movement measure, the right hemisphere,
|
||
|
and creativity. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 823-830.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Fisk, J. & Rourke, B. (1979) Identification of subtypes of learning
|
||
|
disabled children at three age levels: A neuropsychological,
|
||
|
multivariate approach. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1,
|
||
|
289-310.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Furst, C. EEG asymmetry and visuospatial performance. Nature,
|
||
|
260, 254-255. Galin,D., & Ornstein, R. (1972) Lateral
|
||
|
specialization of cognitive mode: An EEG study.
|
||
|
Psychophysiology, 9, 412-418.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Galin, D., & Ornstein, R. (1975) Hemispheric specialization and
|
||
|
the duality of consciousness. Laterality and brain function.
|
||
|
Springfield, IL: C.C.Thomas.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Golden, M., & Zenhausern, R. (1983)
|
||
|
Grapheme to phoneme conversion: The basis of reading
|
||
|
disability? International Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 229-240.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Gur, R.E. (1978) Left hemisphere dysfunction and left hemisphere
|
||
|
overactivation in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal
|
||
|
Psychology, 87, 226-238.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (1975). Defense mechanisms,
|
||
|
psychosomatic symptomatology, and conjugate lateral eye
|
||
|
movements. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
|
||
|
43, 416-420.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Gur, R.E., Gur, R.C., & Harris (1975) Cerebral activation, as
|
||
|
measured by subjects' lateral eye movements is influenced by
|
||
|
experimenter location. Neuropsychologia, 13, 35-44.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Gur, R.C., Packer, I, Hungerbuhler, J., Reivich, M., Obrist, W., & Amarner,
|
||
|
W.(1980) Differences in the ion of grey and white matter in the
|
||
|
human cerebral cortex. Science, 207, 1226-128.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Huang, M. S., & Byrne, B. (1978). Cognitive style and lateral eye
|
||
|
movements. British Journal of Psychology, 69, 85-90.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Hugdahl, K., & Carlgren, H. E. (1981). Hemispheric asymmetry as
|
||
|
indexed by differences in direction of initial conjugate
|
||
|
lateral eye movements (CLEMS) in response to verbal, spatial,
|
||
|
and emotional tasks. Journal of Mind and Behavior 2, 259-270.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Jamieson, J. L., & Sellick, T. B. (1985) Effects of subject-to-
|
||
|
experimenter distance and instructions on lateral eye movement.
|
||
|
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 155-159.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Kaliski, M., Zenhausern, R., Andrews, R. (1989) The direct
|
||
|
access method of teaching reading and the diagnosis and
|
||
|
remediation of reading disability. International Journal
|
||
|
of Neuroscience, 44, 103-109.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Kinsbourne, M. (1972) Eye and head turning indicate cerebral
|
||
|
lateralization. Science, 176, 539-541.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Kinsbourne, M., & Warrington, E. (1963) Developmental factors in
|
||
|
reading and writing backwardness. British Journal of
|
||
|
Psychology, 54, 145-156.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Krikorian, R., & Rafales, L. (1983). Emotional stimulation,
|
||
|
defensive orientation, and hemispheric activation. Brain and
|
||
|
Cognition, 1, 371-380.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Leboeuf, A., McKay, P., & Clarke, K. (1983). Lateral eye movements
|
||
|
and dream recall in males: a reappraisal. Imagination,
|
||
|
Cognition, and Personality, 3, 61-68.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Lenhart, R. E. (1985) The effects of distance between interactants
|
||
|
and subject anxiety on conjugate lateral eye movements.
|
||
|
Brain and Cognition, 4(3), 328-337.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Levy, J. (1974) Psychobiological implications of bilateral
|
||
|
asymmetry. In S. Dimond & J. Beaumont (Eds.) Hemispheric
|
||
|
function and the human brain. New York: Wiley
|
||
|
|
||
|
Markman, S. & Zenhausern, R. (1988) The interaction of hemispheric related
|
||
|
strategies and individual differences in learning.
|
||
|
International Journal of Neuroscience, 43, 165-170.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Mattis, S., French, J., & Rapin, I. (1975) Dyslexia in children and young
|
||
|
adults: three independent neuropsychological syndromes.
|
||
|
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 17, 150-163
|
||
|
|
||
|
Maxwell, M. & Zenhausern, R. (1983) Teaching reading to disabled
|
||
|
readers by eliminating the necessity for grapheme to phoneme
|
||
|
conversion. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological
|
||
|
Association, Philadelphia.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Minardi, A., Zenhausern, R., & Maxwell, M. (1984) The remediation
|
||
|
of reading disability: a new approach. Paper presented at the
|
||
|
International Neuropsychology Convention, Houston.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Oexle, J. & Zenhausern, R. (1981) Differential hemispheric
|
||
|
activation in good and poor readers. International Journal of
|
||
|
Neuroscience, 15, 31-36.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Ogorman, J. & Siddle, D. (1981). The effects of question type and
|
||
|
experimenter position on bilateral differences in electrodermal
|
||
|
activity and conjugate lateral eye movements. Acta
|
||
|
Psychologica, 49, 43-51.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Owens, W., & Limber, J. (1983). Lateral eye movement as a measure
|
||
|
of cognitive ability and style. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
|
||
|
56, 711-719.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Parrott, C. A. (1983). Personality characteristics associated with
|
||
|
lateral eye movement patterns. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
|
||
|
58, 867-874.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Paivio, A., Yuille, J., & Madigan, S. (1968) Concreteness, imagery
|
||
|
and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of
|
||
|
Experimental Psychology Monographs, 76, (1 pt. 2).
|
||
|
|
||
|
Petroskas, R. & Rourke, B. (1979) Identification of subtypes
|
||
|
of retarded readers: A neuropsychological, multivariate approach.
|
||
|
Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1, 17-37.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Pirozzolo, F. (1979) The neuropsychology of developmental reading
|
||
|
disorders. New York: Praeger.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Schwartz, G., Davidson, R., & Maer, F. (1977). Right Hemisphere
|
||
|
Lateralization for Emotion in the Human Brain: Interactions
|
||
|
with cognition. Science, 190, 286-288.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Schweitzer, L. (1979). Differences of cerebral lateralization
|
||
|
among schizophrenic and depressed patients. Biological
|
||
|
Psychiatry, 14, 721-733.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Smokler, I. A., & Shevrin, H. (1979). Cerebral lateralization and
|
||
|
personality style. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36,
|
||
|
949-954.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Swinnen, S. (1984). Some evidence for the hemispheric asymmetry
|
||
|
model of lateral eye movements. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
|
||
|
58, 79-88.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Thompson, M., Greenberg, R. P., & Fisher, S. (1982). Defense
|
||
|
mechanisms, somatic symptoms, and lateral eye movements in
|
||
|
females. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 939-942.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Tucker, D. M., Antes, J. R., Stenslie, C. E., & Barnhardt, T. M.
|
||
|
(1978). Anxiety and lateral cerebral function. Journal of
|
||
|
Abnormal Psychology, 87, 380-383.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Tucker, G. H., & Suib, M. R. (1978). Conjugate lateral
|
||
|
eye movement (CLEM) direction and its relationship to
|
||
|
performance on verbal and visuospatial tasks.
|
||
|
Neuropsychologica, 16, 251-254
|
||
|
|
||
|
Tucker, D. (1981) Lateral brain function, emotion, and
|
||
|
conceptualization. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 19-46.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Van Nuys, D. (1985) Lateral eye movement and dream recall: II sex
|
||
|
differences and handedness. International Journal of
|
||
|
Psychosomatics, 31, 3-7.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Weiten, W., & Etaugh, C. F. (1974) Lateral eye movement as related
|
||
|
to verbal and perceptual-motor skills and values. Perceptual
|
||
|
and Motor Skills, 36, 423-428.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. (1978) Imagery, cerebral dominance, and
|
||
|
style of thinking: A unified field model. Bulletin of
|
||
|
the Psychonomic Society, 12, 381-384.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. (1982) Rights and lefts and how they learn. Early
|
||
|
Years, 12, 51-67.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. (1983) Lateralized presentation
|
||
|
and hemispheric integration. American Psychologist, 38.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. (1987) The relationship of reading disability and
|
||
|
the techniques of teaching reading. International Journal of
|
||
|
Neuroscience, 34, 55-62.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. (1988) Eyes Right. Teaching K through 8, 23, 76-78.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. & Dunivin, D. (1981) Differential hemispheric
|
||
|
activation and handedness and hysterical and obsessive
|
||
|
personality styles. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 17,
|
||
|
23-25.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R., & Dunn, R. (1984) How brainy are you about the
|
||
|
brain? Early Years, 15, 46-49.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R., Dunn, R., Cavanaugh, D. & Eberle, B. (1981) Do
|
||
|
right and left brained students learn differently. Roeper
|
||
|
Review, 4, 35-39.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. & Nickel, M. (1978) Hemispheric dominance in recall
|
||
|
and recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 13, 50-54.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. & Gebhard, M. (1979) Hemispheric dominance in recall
|
||
|
and recognition. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 14, 71-73.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R., Notaro, J. Grosso, J. & Schiano, P. (1980) The
|
||
|
interaction of hemispheric preference, laterality, and sex in
|
||
|
the perception of emotional tone and verbal content.
|
||
|
International Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 121-126.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. & Parisi, I. (1981) Brain dis-integration in
|
||
|
psychosis. Paper presented at International Neuropsychology
|
||
|
Society, Atlanta.
|
||
|
|
||
|
Zenhausern, R. & Sinatra, R.(1983) The rapid identification of poor
|
||
|
readers with a grapheme to phoneme deficit. Eastern
|
||
|
Psychological Association, Philadelphia.
|
||
|
|
||
|
----
|
||
|
For more information contact:
|
||
|
Robert Zenhausern, Ph.D. Internet: drz@sjuvm.stjohns.edu
|
||
|
St. John's University Bitnet: drz@sjuvm.bitnet
|
||
|
SB 15 Marillac Phone: 718-990-6447
|
||
|
Jamaica, NY 11439 Fax: 718-990-6705
|